Skip to main content

Systematic review of control groups in nutrition education intervention research

Abstract

Background

Well-designed research trials are critical for determining the efficacy and effectiveness of nutrition education interventions. To determine whether behavioral and/or cognition changes can be attributed to an intervention, the experimental design must include a control or comparison condition against which outcomes from the experimental group can be compared. Despite the impact different types of control groups can have on study outcomes, the treatment provided to participants in the control condition has received limited attention in the literature.

Methods

A systematic review of control groups in nutrition education interventions was conducted to better understand how control conditions are described in peer-reviewed journal articles compared with experimental conditions. To be included in the systematic review, articles had to be indexed in CINAHL, PubMed, PsycINFO, WoS, and/or ERIC and report primary research findings of controlled nutrition education intervention trials conducted in the United States with free-living consumer populations and published in English between January 2005 and December 2015. Key elements extracted during data collection included treatment provided to the experimental and control groups (e.g., overall intervention content, tailoring methods, delivery mode, format, duration, setting, and session descriptions, and procedures for standardizing, fidelity of implementation, and blinding); rationale for control group type selected; sample size and attrition; and theoretical foundation.

Results

The search yielded 43 publications; about one-third of these had an inactive control condition, which is considered a weak study design. Nearly two-thirds of reviewed studies had an active control condition considered a stronger research design; however, many failed to report one or more key elements of the intervention, especially for the control condition. None of the experimental and control group treatments were sufficiently detailed to permit replication of the nutrition education interventions studied.

Conclusions

Findings advocate for improved intervention study design and more complete reporting of nutrition education interventions.

Background

A major goal of nutrition education research is to elucidate factors that enable individuals to improve diet-related behaviors and/or cognitions associated with better health and greater longevity. These factors can then be incorporated in educational and health promotion interventions which, in turn, can be evaluated to determine whether the intervention effects change behaviors and/or cognitions among those assigned to the intervention vs. those in a control condition.

Well-designed research trials are critical for determining the efficacy and effectiveness of new interventions [1]. The basic components of educational research intervention trials include experimental variables, such as a novel curriculum; strong, measurable research questions or hypotheses; valid and reliable instruments for documenting change in behavior and/or cognitions; a strong data analysis plan; and an experimental design that minimizes threats to internal validity. To determine whether behavioral and/or cognition changes can be attributed to the intervention, the experimental design must include a control or comparison condition against which outcomes from the experimental group can be compared [2,3,4,5]. The randomized controlled trial (RCT) is typically considered the “gold standard” for ascertaining intervention efficacy and effectiveness [2].

Experts emphasize that to robustly minimize biases and variability of factors that may influence intervention trial outcomes, the control and experimental conditions must: 1) contain randomly assigned participants; 2) occur simultaneously to ensure both conditions experience the same history (i.e., external events, such as political change, natural disasters, scientific discoveries) and maturation (i.e., internal events, such as physical growth, memory decline with aging); 3) be structurally equivalent on as many non-specific factors as possible (i.e., factors other than the “active” ingredients in the experimental condition, such as participant time commitment, format and timeline of activities and data collection, and extent of attention and support from research staff) [5]; and 4) offer equal value, attractiveness, credibility, and outcome expectations to keep participants blind to their condition assignment and thereby avoid novelty effects, differential dropout rates, disappointment arising from assignment to the control group, and/or efforts by control group participants to seek an alternate source of the treatment offered to the experimental group [1, 3, 4, 6,7,8,9,10,11,12,13,14,15,16]. The control condition also must not modify the intervention’s specific factors (i.e., behavior and/or cognitions targeted in the experimental condition) [4, 7].

To reduce the risk of a Type 1 error (acceptance of an ineffective intervention) [1, 9, 17], treatment received by control condition participants should differ from those in the experimental condition only in the non-receipt of the “active ingredient” of the intervention hypothesized to affect study outcomes [4, 6]. Rigorous control of non-specific factors, however, tends to increase intervention research costs because a plausible control intervention must be developed and implemented. Additionally, as the stringency of control exerted over non-specific factors increases, the risk of understating the effectiveness of the intervention rises because effect size is inversely associated with rigor of non-specific factor control [9, 17,18,19]. Therefore, to demonstrate statistically meaningful differences, larger sample sizes are needed to avoid Type 2 errors (failure to recognize an intervention is effective) and detect treatment effects when the control and experimental group treatments are structurally equivalent than when a less equivalent control treatment is used [1, 9, 17].

A key challenge to nutrition education researchers is selecting a suitable treatment for the control condition that is congruent with the research question, study resources, availability of standard treatment/usual care, and ethical considerations [7, 9, 10, 12, 20, 21]. Control condition participants may receive treatment ranging from nothing at all to extensive treatment in an alternate “active” control condition unrelated to the experimental condition. As indicated in Table 1, the type of control condition selected can have important effects on study resources, participants, internal validity, and outcomes. For instance, resource investment in the treatment for the control condition can range from zero for the inactive control to considerable for active control. Ethical issues may be more highly problematic in inactive control conditions when participants in need of the intervention are denied treatment, but ethical issues are lessened when a standard or usual treatment can be offered. Preventing disappointed control group participants from seeking alternate sources of the treatment may not be possible, which weakens internal validity and undermines a true evaluation of the intervention’s effect. Even in active control conditions where participants receive a contemporaneous intervention equal to the treatment condition in all aspects, except the “active ingredient”, researchers may inadvertently treat control participants differently. Those delivering the intervention (e.g., research staff, educators) also may dislike being in the control condition [22] and seek opportunities to provide participants with treatment like that being given to the experimental group.

Table 1 Control Condition Treatments

Clearly, the efficacy and “effectiveness of the experimental condition inherently depends as much on the control condition as on the experimental condition” [1],p.276. Despite the impact different types of control groups can have on study outcomes [23], the treatment provided to participants in the control condition has received limited attention in the literature [1, 7, 12, 17, 20, 24,25,26] and sometimes is not even described in research designs [27, 28]; yet in the words of Mohr et al. with regard to psychological interventions, “inappropriate control conditions can overestimate the effectiveness of a treatment, or kill off a potentially useful treatment” [1],p.283. Thus, a systematic review of control groups in nutrition education interventions was conducted with the goal of better understanding how control conditions are described in peer-reviewed primary outcomes journal articles in comparison with experimental conditions. An additional goal of this investigation is to open discussions among colleagues as to how best to improve reporting of control and experimental condition treatments in intervention evaluation studies to facilitate advancement of the field.

Methods

A systematic literature search was conducted after review of guidance from the Nutrition Education Systematic Review Project [29]. The study team then identified databases to use in the systematic review, search terms, and inclusion and exclusion criteria.

Search strategies

Search strategies were formulated according to the PRISMA guidelines [30]. Subject headings or search terms unique to each database were identified and searched in combination with keywords derived from the major concepts of “nutrition education intervention” and “control groups” or “study design”. Table 2 shows the final search strategy for the selected databases (i.e., CINAHL, PubMed, PsycINFO, WoS, and ERIC). Searches were conducted in winter 2016.

Table 2 Search strategies for databases searcheda

To be included in the systematic review, the articles had to report primary research findings of controlled nutrition education intervention trials from peer-reviewed journals. Included studies could address content other than nutrition, but nutrition had to be a key component. Additionally, included interventions had to focus on health promotion and disease prevention and have an education component. Inclusion criteria also required that interventions consist of more than one session and be conducted in the United States with free-living consumer populations. All included articles were published in English between January 2005 and December 2015. In cases where more than one article from the same study was located, only primary outcomes paper was included in the review to prevent over-representation of the type of control group used.

Excluded articles were studies reporting pilot, feasibility, cross-sectional, follow-up, or secondary analysis findings and those lacking a control or comparison group. Studies that focused on weight loss or disease management/treatment and those lacking an education component (e.g., those solely manipulating environmental factors) also were excluded. Additionally, all studies targeting professionals (e.g., health care, child care) or individuals recruited due to a pre-existing disease, such as diabetes, eating disorders, and obesity, or hospitalization, were excluded.

Data management

Citations for the 1164 articles returned by the systematic literature search were entered in a citation management tool (Fig. 1). After removal of duplicates (n = 46) and publications that were not complete primary research articles (e.g., commentaries, viewpoints, editorials, letters, survey studies, abstracts, review articles, n = 50), two members of the study team independently conducted an initial screening of all article titles to identify those congruent with the study purpose. The title review yielded 195 articles that appeared to meet inclusion criteria. Next, article abstracts were independently reviewed by the same team members and 83 were identified as congruent with study purposes. Four team members scanned the articles and identified 53 articles meeting inclusion criteria. During data extraction, 10 additional articles were eliminated because they did not meet inclusion criteria thereby yielding a total of 43 reviewed articles.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flow chart of literature search results for controlled research studies reporting (e.g., not secondary analysis or pilot, feasibility, or follow-up studies) results of nutrition education primary-prevention (e.g., not part of treatment for disease or weight loss) interventions consisting of more than one session conducted with free-living individuals in the United States

Data collection and analysis

After scrutinizing guidance from the Nutrition Education Systematic Review Project [29] and Cochrane Collaboration [31, 32] as well as previously published systematic reviews [33,34,35], data extraction tables were designed by the study team. These tables were iteratively pilot-tested and refined.

Data were extracted by one team member and independently checked for accuracy by two other team members. As shown in Table 3, the factors extracted included treatment provided to the experimental and control groups, overall intervention content, procedures used to tailor the intervention to participants, intervention delivery mode (e.g., group, individual), intervention format (e.g., curriculum, website, brochure) and duration, intervention setting, individual intervention session description (e.g., number of sessions or interactions, session duration, session frequency, content of each session, time allotment for each session component, overall duration of the intervention), procedures for standardizing intervention across multiple sites/practitioners, procedures for assessing fidelity of implementation across multiple sites, and procedures for blinding (masking) participants and/or intervention staff to participant group assignment, rationale for control group type selected, as well as sample size, attrition rate, and theoretical foundation. The goal of the factors extracted was to document the explicit presence or absence of each factor reported in the article. Additionally, only the 43 articles identified in the search were reviewed; extracting additional data from bibliographical references to previous developmental work cited in articles was beyond the scope of this study. A written narrative describing the treatment groups was prepared for each study. Extraction tables were content analyzed by team members to identify themes used to prepare a narrative synthesis of findings.

Table 3 Factors Extracted in Systematic Reviewa

Results

The treatment provided to the experimental and control conditions in the studies meeting the inclusion criteria are described in Table 4. For accuracy, these descriptions used verbiage from the original research inasmuch as possible [36]. More than one-third of the 43 studies in the review had an inactive control condition; that is, the control group received no treatment or delayed-treatment (or wait-list). Because a key goal of this study was to compare how control and experimental conditions are described in peer-reviewed literature, results will focus on the 28 studies that had an active control condition. Of these studies, 7 had a usual or standard treatment for the control group, 12 offered an alternative active treatment to control participants, and 9 were dismantling (or additive) component active controls (2 of the 9 were mixed in that control groups received an alternative active treatment whereas the experimental groups received additive treatments).

Table 4 Description of experimental and control group treatments of nutrition education interventions (n = 43)

Factors extracted in reviewed articles

Additional file 1 Table S5 compares the presence of factors extracted in the systematic review of articles. Each factor is described below, citing examples of studies demonstrating the factor

Description of overall intervention content

Reviewed articles commonly included a description of the overall intervention content provided. Content tended to focus on increasing fruit and/or vegetable intake, lowering fat intake, and healthy eating in general. The extensiveness of the overall content description for experimental groups ranged from only naming the general topic area (e.g., fruits and vegetables) [37] to listing topics and content addressed [38, 39] to reporting content and participant activities [40,41,42] and teaching strategies [43,44,45,46].

Descriptions of the overall content for the control conditions tended to provide much less detail compared to experimental conditions. For example, among those employing usual or standard treatment, one study indicated only that “control classrooms did not receive vegetable-related instruction” [40],p.39 whereas another study reported that health education with no nutrition content was given [43], with neither indicating what control group participants received. Other descriptions of the control condition of usual treatment studies were equally vague indicating these participants received “traditional”, “regular”, or “normal” lessons [37, 38, 41, 47]. Descriptions of treatment provided to the control groups in some alternative active treatment studies also were vague (e.g., control received pamphlet on fruits and vegetables [48], “packet of 5 printed commercially available booklets [49],” videos on sleep disorders [50]). However, several alternative active treatment investigations were more informative, including content similar in detail to the experimental group [46, 49, 51,52,53]. Dismantling studies tended to provide the greatest detail about the control condition largely because most experimental conditions were additive to the base formed by the control.

Description of how the intervention was tailored

Unless a goal of an investigation was to determine the effects of tailoring, little information on this factor was reported for experimental or control conditions regardless of whether a usual or other active control condition was used. In usual treatment control conditions, only one study mentioned tailoring for the experimental group [37]. A few alternative active treatment control condition studies tailored experimental and control treatments to demographic characteristics (e.g., older adult learners, African American women) [51, 52]. Some investigations tailored treatments for experimental groups by allowing participants to choose topics or materials [45, 49], with one study giving both experimental and control groups the ability to select topics [51]. The aim of most dismantling studies was to assess the effects of tailoring (experimental groups) vs not tailoring (control group); thus, tailoring descriptions for the control group generally were not applicable. On the other hand, the relative importance of the tailoring method to study aims made reasonably complete descriptions of this process requisite to report for experimental groups. Gans et al. reported [54] that tailoring was based on participant’s fat, fruit, and vegetable intake and related behaviors, self-identified needed behavior changes, personal motivators, barriers, and other psychosocial issues associated with healthy eating, needs, and interests. Resnicow et al.’s [53] report is notable in that these authors provided a table describing messages and graphic images used to tailor study newsletters.

Description of intervention delivery mode, material type used, duration, and setting

Across all types of control conditions, investigators consistently reported the intervention delivery mode, with the most common being group sessions or online. Descriptions for experimental conditions tended to express delivery mode in explicit terms whereas for control conditions, it was often left to the reader to decide on the mode using implicit clues. This was particularly the case when the control group received a “usual” treatment without further clarification [40, 41, 43, 47, 55].

The type of material that provided intervention content directed to participants tended to be printed (e.g., brochures, pamphlets, manuals, newsletters) and online (e.g., websites, videos). Interventions delivered by instructors to groups used mostly curricula and “lessons.” Some of the reviewed articles gave bibliographical references, internet links, or other means for obtaining intervention materials, with sources for instructional materials more commonly given for experimental than control groups [38, 40,41,42,43, 47, 55,56,57,58,59]. An examination by control group type found that references for resources used to deliver usual treatment to control groups were not included. Among alternative active treatment studies, the material types used with both experimental and control groups had comparably detailed descriptions [39, 42, 51, 60], with some exceptions where great detail about the materials used by the experimental group was provided while giving only limited descriptions of those intended for the control group [44, 48]. Material type descriptions tended to be more even across dismantling studies.

Total duration of the intervention delivered to the experimental group was explicitly stated in nearly all studies reviewed. For control groups, total duration was less likely to be clearly described and frequently had to be deduced from a review of the study timeline (e.g., when the baseline and post-test was administered) and comparison to statements made about the experimental group. The setting where group sessions were delivered normally was overtly indicated (e.g., school, community center). Interventions directed to individuals who received mailed materials or used websites generally only implied the setting as being home or worksite [49, 50, 56, 57] and did not report where participants generally used intervention materials.

Description of individual intervention sessions

Across all types of control groups, the number of sessions or interactions (e.g., newsletters) usually was explicitly stated for both treatment groups. The duration of individual sessions or length of materials was more commonly reported for experimental than usual treatment control groups; for other types of control groups, duration was somewhat more consistently reported for both treatment groups [48, 61]. Reporting of frequency of sessions was fairly even across experimental and control groups in all types of control conditions except usual treatment, where this information was rarely included.

Reports of the content of individual sessions/interactions were provided in about half the active control articles reviewed with most descriptions being abbreviated for the experimental group and virtually non-existent for the control group. In a few cases, researchers provided a table or figure listing concepts/topics/objectives addressed in each session/interaction for the experimental group [40, 41, 54, 61, 62]. Only 2 studies provided a table describing the content of both the experimental and control treatments [46, 49]. Descriptions of the duration of each main component of individual sessions/interactions were rare. The exceptions were Ratcliffe et al. [61] who stated “[e]ach hour-long session consisted of approximately 20 min of instruction followed by 40 min of hands-on garden experiences”p.38, Herbert et al. [38] who reported “Energize engages children in 1, 60-minute class once a week … by involving them in 15 minutes of nutrition education, a 10-minute warm-up … and 35 minutes of aerobic exercise activities and fitness games”p.781, and Pobocik et al. [41] who indicated “[a]pproximately 20 minutes of the 45-minute class were allotted to presenting information … remaining time … for testing, activities, and demonstrations”p.22. Comparable descriptions for control groups were not included.

Procedures for standardization across centers/practitioners

Procedures for standardizing the experimental condition intervention delivery across centers/practitioners took several forms, including training instructors [38, 40, 43, 45, 47, 52, 55] and utilizing pre-established curricula (instructional lessons and protocols) [38, 40, 41, 43, 47, 55] and/or instructional materials (e.g., printed materials, videos, websites) [37, 48,49,50, 56, 57]. Standardization procedures were similarly addressed across types of interventions for the experimental group. In contrast, little information related to standardization of implementation of control group treatments was provided for usual treatment control conditions. In alternative and dismantling active treatment studies, the procedures for standardizing control group treatment were frequently addressed and mostly took the form of pre-established instructional materials [39, 49, 50, 52,53,54, 56, 57, 59, 63,64,65].

Procedures for assessing fidelity of implementation

Only about half of active control studies addressed fidelity of adherence to procedures, with most of these including information about procedures for both the experimental and control conditions. Methods used to establish fidelity of implementation for both experimental and control groups in active control studies where teachers or instructors delivered the treatment included detailed/scripted presentations [43, 46], frequent meetings with researchers [38, 46, 47], random observation/videotaping of instructors [43, 46, 55], teaching/feedback logs [43, 52], and audiotaping [57]. Methods used in active control group studies in which participants self-directed their engagement with pre-established treatments (e.g., web-based, printed materials) included completing forms documenting usage of treatment materials immediately after use [50, 64, 65], self-report posttest survey items that gauged extent of treatment use [53, 58], and website tracking data [59].

The vast majority of active control studies provided little detail about fidelity procedures. One notable exception was McCaughtry et al. [43], who described fidelity procedures as including “very detailed (nearly scripted) lessons in the curriculum…a research assistant [who] conducted randomized school visits to observe each health education teacher’s instruction to guarantee that the control teachers were not teaching nutrition content and that the intervention teachers were implementing the curriculum with fidelity,”p.279. Another noteworthy example was provided by Wolf et al.: “Treatment fidelity checks were conducted on 200 (41%) of the intervention calls. Trained raters listened to audio recordings of the calls and completed a checklist documenting whether specific points were covered and whether the interventionist spoke at an appropriate pace, responded to questions with clear answers and probed at appropriate times” [57],p.34.

Procedures for blinding participants and researchers to treatment group assignment

Limited attention was given to the issue of blinding participants or researchers in the reviewed articles. In many cases, it was not clear whether participants were blinded (or aware there was a control vs experimental group), although this is a typical component of informed consent procedures. None of the studies providing the control group with usual treatment addressed participant blinding. Two articles blinded participants to group assignment by explaining that they were getting one of two programs or using alternate names for “control” or “experimental” groups. In specific, McCarthy et al. stated “A portion of the script used by project staff read … This is a cancer prevention study to compare two programs designed to help black women reduce their risk of cancer and improve their appearance. The first program involves 8 weekly 2-h sessions on diet and exercise. The second program involves 8 weekly 2-h sessions on current health topics of interest to black women, such as breast cancer and menopause. Both programs will be conducted by black women physicians and other professionals. We'll decide which group you'll be assigned to randomly, for example, by flipping a coin…” [51], p.247. In McClelland et al.’s crossover design study, these researchers assigned participants “to either the Apples Group (n=6) with the treatment curriculum … delivered first or the Beans Group (n=7) with the control curriculum … delivered first” [42], p.2. Another study reported that participant blinding efforts may not have worked. These researchers stated that “[g]irls, mothers, and troop leaders were masked to their group membership assignment;” but went on to say “because the project was called the Osteoporosis Prevention Project, some individuals in the control troops may have determined their status owing to the generic health focus of the sessions” [46],p.158.

The issue of blinding research staff likely is less important when interventions are automated and participant exposure to staff is minimal or non-existent. However, even when there was significant interaction with staff (e.g., in interventions delivering in-person or phone-based treatments), studies rarely addressed staff blinding. A few investigators reported using different instructors for experimental and control conditions [51, 52], whereas others indicated that instructors were not blind to condition due to the nature of the intervention [46, 55, 57]. Blinding also would have been difficult in some of the dismantling studies where part of the treatment for only one of multiple experimental groups involved live interactions with staff [59, 63]. In a few cases, articles reported that study evaluators were kept blind to participant study group assignment [57, 58, 64].

Rationale for selection of control group type

Reviewed studies seldom provided a rationale for the type of control group used and for those that did, various reasons were cited. These included convenience and comparability (e.g., “Three comparison [college] courses … were selected because they also were upper-level Human Biology courses, were delivered the same quarter, and were taught by experienced health promotion researchers and focused on a health message” [44], p.544) and relative strength (e.g., “Control group participants received fewer follow-up mailings … [that] resulted in a difference in “attention” between treatment arms, it is nonetheless a stronger design than a no-treatment control group” [60], p.62). Appropriateness to setting and participants also was considered (e.g., “Employees … were … assigned to the Web-based … or the print condition. It was recognized that the print materials could also be effective instruments of health behavior improvement (unlike a no-treatment control group) and could be a challenge as a control group … [and] would be a likely workplace alternative to an online program; therefore, the print group was thought to be an appropriate control group for the study” [49], p.e17). Yet, after finding both interventions yielded similar improvements, the article added to the control group rationale by stating… “[b]ecause it was originally thought that the print materials would form a relatively weak intervention compared to the Web program, a no-treatment control was not included in the design” [49],p.e17. Only 3 studies indicated the rationale for the control group was to control for non-specific effects (i.e.,“[t]he control group provided an intervention of identical intensity and program delivery format as the experimental group, ruling out “attention” effects in the experimental group” [52],p.386, “we used an attention control group to take into account the effect of participation” [65],p.37, and “[t]he purpose of this group was to control for any nonspecific effects from being educated about healthy lifestyles and from contact time and number of sessions … with professionals [46],p.158.”

Behavior change theory use

Nearly one-quarter of all reviewed studies did not indicate whether a theory was used to guide the intervention. Of those that indicated application of a behavior change theory, more than half used the social cognitive theory and about one-quarter used the transtheoretical model. Most studies named the theory used with little additional explanation of how it was operationalized. The most explicit reporting of theory application was by Pobocik et al. [41], who included a table listing social cognitive theory constructs, definition of the construct, and an example of how the construct was operationalized in the Do Dairy intervention. Of those reporting how theories were applied, several used the stage of change construct for tailoring materials [48, 63, 66] and/or selecting assessment scales [40, 48, 50, 54, 64]. Particularly illustrative of theory use in assessment were the tables Wall et al. [40] and Elder et al. [64] provided that listed theory constructs and corresponding evaluation items.

Comparison across control condition types

In the 7 investigations using a usual or standard active control condition, consisting of “traditional” or “regular” instruction, participants tended to be children enrolled in school or participants in government sponsored programs—perhaps because these systems have an ongoing program available for comparison. Articles gave fairly complete descriptions of the intervention provided to the experimental group, which were mostly curriculum based. They tended not to indicate if or how interventions were tailored and rarely provided information on the content of each session/interaction or how time was apportioned in each session, although this information may be available in the curricula referenced. With regard to the control group intervention, other than the overall intervention content, delivery (individual or group), and setting, little other information was provided. In most cases, too little information was provided about the usual treatment to determine whether the control group’s treatment was comparable on non-specific factors to that received by the experimental group [38, 40, 41, 47, 55]. Descriptions in one study, which compared differences in teaching strategies (e.g., traditional vs. tailored online) indicated fairly similar attention to non-specific factors [37].

In the 12 studies providing an alternative active treatment to the control group, investigators included a fairly even description of the treatments given to both experimental and control groups—a notable exception for both groups was a lack of specificity regarding the amount of time in each session devoted to the main components of the treatment. Additionally, many of the interventions were mail- or web-based and did not explicitly indicate the intervention setting. A comparison of the intensity of the treatments offered indicates that in some studies, the control group received “lighter” treatment doses than the experimental group (e.g., control group received a single pamphlet whereas the experimental group received tailored monthly magazines for 8 months [48], packet of printed booklets vs. highly interactive web-based program [49], manual vs manual coupled with coaching calls, tailored newsletters, and personalized feedback [56]). Many studies appeared comparable across a range of non-specific factors that could affect study outcomes [42, 51, 52]. One example of comparable treatment is Wolf et al. [57] who provided both experimental and control groups with a brochure (different topics) and tailored telephone education. Healy et al. [39] offers a second example in which both groups received a treatment that was the same length of time (7 50-min sessions over 1.5 weeks), used similar teaching strategies (i.e., lecture, discussion, question/answer, group activities), and differed only on content taught.

The 7 dismantling (additive) component active control studies tended to have 2 or more experimental groups. Interestingly, in all but one of these studies, the differences between the experimental and control treatments hinged on tailoring [61]. The control, or comparison, group in nearly all of these studies received less personalized and less intensive treatment than the experimental group [54, 59, 61, 63, 64]. In one study, for example, 3 groups of women either received non-tailored newsletters, tailored newsletters, or tailored newsletters and visits with lay health advisors [64]. Because of the derivative nature and increasing intensity of treatment provided by most dismantling studies [54, 59, 61, 63, 64], there was an imbalance in non-specific factors between/among study groups. The in-person and frequent phone contact received by one experimental group vs ongoing access to the project website and automated individual risk profiling given to a second experimental group vs printed materials provided one time to control participants demonstrated the imbalanced attention across study groups [59]. Among dismantling studies, the greatest balance in non-specific effects was achieved by Resnicow et al. [53] in that both experimental and control groups received the same newsletters except the tailoring of the experimental newsletters was more specific.

An additional two dismantling studies were classified as “mixed” [46, 65] because the control participants received an alternate treatment that was not a derivative of the experimental group but was similar to treatment provided to control participants in alternative active treatment conditions. For instance, control condition participants in one study received 2 45-min web sessions on anatomy whereas those in the 2 experimental groups received 2 45-min web sessions on nutrition or 2 45-min web sessions plus a 45-min booster session [65]. The comparability of treatment provided to control groups in these 2 mixed dismantling active control studies tended to be more balanced on non-specific factors than the other 7 dismantling studies that did not have an alternative treatment.

Other findings

Reports of sample size and attrition were uneven. Some studies provided a complete description of total numbers recruited and retained, by treatment group, at each phase of the study [55, 67], with several including CONSORT diagrams [48, 53,54,55, 57,58,59, 63, 65, 66, 68, 69]. However, other studies only reported sample sizes at baseline [70], posttest [43, 71, 72], those completing both pretest and posttest [22, 45], or sample sizes and/or attrition rates for both groups combined [41, 73].

More than 3 out of 4 studies reviewed had random assignment of participants or intact groups (e.g., classrooms). Of the 10 non-randomized trials, half had no treatment control conditions. Of the remainder, one did not address randomization [41], one indicated the experimental group was comprised of students in classrooms with teachers who volunteered to participate [38], and another involving college students used intact classes and did not randomize the classes [44]. Two studies offered more explanation. One that was offered in WIC clinics indicated randomization was impractical and stated that “the practicality of being able to actually study comparisons of nutrition education intervention modalities in a typical clinic setting overcompensated for the lack in ability to develop a randomized design” [37],p.754. Authors of the second study offered this rationale, “The high cost and limited availability of randomized controlled trials in community settings highlight a need to evaluate and report on nonrandomized interventions that can be implemented in existing community settings” [45],p. 265.

Terminology used to describe control groups was not always consistent with definitions in Table 1. For example, two papers referred to control groups who received usual instruction as no treatment controls [37, 43]. Another provided an alternative active treatment, yet referred to it as a standard treatment [48]. Still another referred to the alternative active treatment control group as an attention placebo group [65]. A placebo should have no effect on a person, however because learning likely occurred in this and other alternate education-related control conditions, the term placebo does not accurately describe the control condition.

Discussion

The goal of this study was to conduct a systematic review of control groups in nutrition education interventions and describe how control conditions are reported in peer-reviewed primary outcomes journal articles in comparison with experimental conditions. The findings of this systematic review indicate that the articles sampled focused on a wide array of controlled nutrition education intervention studies. Most addressed fruits and vegetables, fat intake, and healthy eating and tended to target school children as well as limited resource youth and families enrolled in government sponsored programs. Overall, descriptions of experimental conditions, regardless of type of active control condition, tended to be far more complete than descriptions of control conditions. Studies tended to report nearly all key factors (i.e., intervention content, delivery mode, material type, total duration, setting, individual session/interaction components [e.g.,, number, duration or length, frequency, content], standardization procedures, procedures for assessing fidelity of implementation, references for materials, theoretical underpinnings, and randomization) for the experimental condition. However, descriptions of the experimental group commonly lacked procedures for blinding and tailoring (except when the study was comparing differences in the effect of tailoring). In contrast, control conditions lacked descriptions of many key factors, with the most commonly omitted factors being individual sessions/interactions (e.g., number, duration, frequency, content of individual sessions), procedures for standardization, procedures for assessing implementation fidelity, blinding procedures, rationale for the type of control group selected, and references for instructional materials. Additionally, the factors that were reported for control conditions tended to be less explicit and included fewer details than provided for the experimental condition. In many cases, too little information was provided to determine the comparability of the control group vis-à-vis non-specific factors. Overall, the descriptions of both control and experimental group treatments became more complete as the type of active control became stronger and more complex; that is, alternative active treatments and dismantling studies provided the most detailed descriptions of the control group condition whereas usual or standard control conditions provided the least detail.

One-third of the 43 reviewed studies had inactive control conditions (i.e., no treatment or delayed treatment), a research design that is considered weak [7, 17]. The Food and Drug Administration instructs that a no-treatment control be used only when investigation outcomes are entirely objective and cannot be biased by lack of blinding [74]—although this advice is directed at drug trials, it can be reasonably applied to education trials using inactive controls. For instance, in one delayed treatment study, researchers stated that a lack of blinding among those teaching the educational intervention was problematic (i.e., they “generally did not like to be randomized to the control condition [22],p.31”). Failure to implement procedures to prevent differential treatment, commitment, and engagement of both experimental and control condition instructors has the potential to confound results [75]. Likely many researchers conducting the 43 reviewed studies had implemented appropriate blinding procedures for participants, instructors, and researchers; however descriptions of procedures for blinding and/or prevention of differential treatment were not reported in most studies.

Active control conditions, considered a stronger research design than inactive [7], were used in two-thirds of the reviewed studies. In this and other studies [76], usual treatment was considered an active control whereas some researchers categorize usual treatment as inactive (or passive) because it typically is not structurally equivalent on non-specific factors to the experimental condition [6, 7, 24, 32, 76]. All usual treatment conditions in reviewed studies offered control groups traditional or regular instruction that did not include content offered to the experimental group. As Street and Luoma point out, it usually is not possible to equalize all non-specific factors (particularly credibility and outcome expectations) when using education about an unrelated topic as the usual treatment [6]. The limited information about the usual treatment given to control participants negated the possibility of confidently affirming equivalency of intensity and structure of control and experimental treatments.

A hallmark of evidence building is replicability. Similar to findings by researchers in other fields [12, 26], none of the experimental and control group treatments were sufficiently detailed to permit replication of the nutrition education interventions studied. About half of the experimental treatment descriptions included a reference for the intervention materials and a third of the control treatment descriptions included this information; these materials may mitigate replication issues associated with missing information in the reviewed article. Another alternative is to contact authors to obtain intervention details. When Glasziou et al. contacted authors who published non-pharmacological medical treatment intervention outcomes, treatment descriptions improved significantly; however one-third of the studies they reviewed still had insufficient detail, in part because study authors did not respond despite repeated attempts or were unwilling to provide additional information [26].

Standardization and fidelity procedures are equally important for control and experimental conditions—without these procedures, either group may receive more or less than the research protocol intended which likely will confound outcomes [75, 77, 78]. The limited reporting of standardization procedures (e.g., use of manuals, standard operating procedures) and process evaluation activities in the reviewed studies, and noted by others in psychological therapy research [77], indicates that either reports are incomplete or these procedures were not implemented—neither of which are helpful when trying to weigh the value of the study outcomes and determine whether treatment groups received differential treatment from unblinded research staff.

Random assignment is considered critical to minimizing biases in trial outcomes and maximizing accuracy of analysis of intervention effects. One-quarter of the reviewed studies did not randomly assign participants, and likely suffered from at least some selection bias [79]. Compounding the lack of randomization is that many of these same studies did not address participant or researcher blinding and/or procedures for assessing intervention implementation fidelity, all of which impair internal validity [79].

Reporting sample size seems like a fairly straightforward task, regardless of how complex an intervention design may be. Indeed, CONSORT flow diagrams [80] make reporting changes in sample size at each stage of the study clear and easy to report. Yet, many of the studies reviewed lacked key sample size information, a phenomenon noted by others [81, 82]. In some cases, sample size was not declared in tables reporting data [38, 47, 51, 72].

It is interesting that so few articles provided a justification for the type of control condition used, especially given this is a conscious decision made during study planning. A systematic review of psychosocial interventions with substance use disorders also found studies gave little justification for control group choice or considerations for how this choice may have affected study outcomes [24].

The classic work of Campbell and Stanley identifies the Solomon 4 group design as offering the greatest internal and external validity checks [5]. This design includes these groups: experimental (pretest-intervention-posttest), no pretest experimental (intervention-posttest), control (pretest-posttest), no pretest control (posttest). Comparison of posttest scores across the 4 groups reveals whether changes are the result of the intervention and/or learning from the test [5, 79]. None of the reviewed studies had non-pretested comparison groups. This lack of control for testing may have important implications; indeed researchers note that repeated measurements may encourage control condition participants to reflect on behaviors and initiate the behavior targeted in the experimental condition [83, 84]. Another research group suggested that the research design for psychosocial treatments that most closely equates to a double-blind design is one that compares “two bona fide interventions … delivered by advocates for those interventions” [24],p. 426–427. In the reviewed studies, just one study met these criteria [57]. That is, Wolf et al. reported that immigrant men were given either a fruit/vegetable or prostate cancer prevention brochure [57]. Both groups received 2 tailored telephone education calls that could be considered to be delivered by an “advocate” because callers use a standardized telephone protocol and were audiotaped as a check for fidelity of delivery (however, no mention was made as to whether different callers were used for each treatment). Still another research group felt that to disentangle effects of the “active ingredient” from effects of non-specific factors, studies should include 3 groups: wait list control, attention control, and experimental group [85]. Many of the reviewed studies had 2 of these groups, but none had all 3.

Dismantling designs make it possible to separately account for the effects of each intervention component. However, the reviewed dismantling studies were mostly additive—that is, the treatment groups received increasingly intensive treatments thereby making it impossible to ascertain whether it was the greater dose of the additive treatment that contributed to changes or just the additional element [46, 54, 58, 63, 65]. For instance, one had a control group who received 12 weekly non-tailored newsletters by mail, an experimental group received 12 weekly tailored newsletters by mail, and another experimental group received the 12 weekly tailored newsletters plus weekly home visits from a promotora (lay health advisor/counselor) [64]. There was not a group who received only promotora visits, thus differentiating between intensity and independent effects of the promotora was not possible.

In the words of Montgomery et al., “[p]oor reporting limits the ability to replicate interventions, synthesise evidence in systematic reviews, and utilise findings for evidence-based policy and practice. The lack of guidance for reporting the specific methodological features of complex intervention RCTs contributes to poor reporting” [86],p.99. To improve reporting, the CONSORT extension underway for randomized controlled trials of social and psychological interventions may be appropriate and/or adaptable for health and nutrition education and promotion programs [86]. Methods for overcoming deficiencies in reporting design and execution of both control and experimental conditions reported by others may serve as models for reporting nutrition education interventions [7, 87]. One research group has even suggested creating a repository of treatment descriptions, citing the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention’s Replicating Effective Programs (https://www.cdc.gov/hiv/research/interventionresearch/rep/index.html) as an example, and establishing a detailed checklist of characteristics to be included in intervention descriptions [26]. In fact, the supplementary table published by Greaves and colleagues is an excellent reporting method that ensures all salient elements are included [87]. Table 3 in this paper is another tool for ensuring key information is reported in nutrition education outcomes papers.

Strengths of this review lie in the large number of papers included and the extensive extraction of data contributing to this comprehensive description of control groups in nutrition education interventions and how they and experimental conditions are recounted in peer-reviewed journals. Additionally, it is the first study to explore control conditions in nutrition education and is among the first in any field to examine this critically important research intervention study design and reporting component [7, 26, 85]. This study is, however, limited to studies conducted in the United States. Furthermore, the studies reviewed likely included at least some of the extracted factors reported as missing in Additional file 1: Table S5, but did not explicitly report them in the published paper. Also, no attempt was made to examine cited sources, which may supplement the information provided in the reviewed papers. Examination of the appropriateness of outcome measures, adequacy of sample sizes, and effect of control condition on study outcomes were beyond the scope of this review, but are important targets for future investigations.

Conclusions

Calls for more transparency and detail in reporting interventions have occurred sporadically since at least 1991, yet little has changed [77, 88]. In this day and age of ever constricting research funding, coupled with the dire need for interventions that effectively improve nutritional status and associated outcomes, it is imperative that intervention research use more robust study designs that permit us to understand the effects of each component of the intervention [26, 85]. Additionally, researchers and journal editors should assume the responsibility for ensuring that practitioners can easily access the details needed to implement effective interventions with fidelity. The key historic barrier to reporting this data in printed form has been overcome with electronic publishing [26, 89]. Clearly there is a great deal of opportunity to improve intervention study design and reporting—seizing this opportunity can only help to advance the field and improve consumer health. A goal set at the outset of the investigation reported here is to open a dialogue among nutrition education researchers that leads to improved reporting of control and experimental condition treatments in intervention evaluation studies to promote advancement and impact of our work.

References

  1. Mohr D, Spring B, Freedland K, Beckner V, Arean P, Hollon S, Ockene J, Kaplan R. The selection and design of control conditions for randomized controlled trials of psychological interventions. Psychother Psychosom. 2009;78:275–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Sullivan G. Getting off the "gold standard": randomized controlled trials and education research. J Grad Med Ed. 2011;3:285–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Fogg L, Gross D. Threats to validity in randomized clinical trials. Research in Nursing & Health. 2000;23:79–87.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Beal CC, Stuifbergen A, Volker D, Becker H. Women's experiences as members of attention control and experimental intervention groups in a randomized controlled trial. Can J Nurs Res. 2009;41(4):16–31.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Campbell D, Stanley J. Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for research. Chicago: Rand McNally College Publishing Company; 1963.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Street L, Louma J. Control groups in psychosocial intervention research: ethical and methodological issues. Ethics & Behavior. 2002;12:1–30.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Lindquist R, Wyman J, Talley K, Findorff M, Gross C. Design of control-group conditions in clinical trials of behavioral interventions. J Nurs Scholarsh. 2007;39:214–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Avenell A, Grant AM, McGee M, McPherson G, Campbell MK, McGee MA. The effects of an open design on trial participant recruitment, compliance and retention--a randomized controlled trial comparison with a blinded, placebo-controlled design. Clin Trials. 2004;1(6):490–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Rethorst CD, Greer TL, Grannemann B, Ring KM, Marcus BH, Trivedi MH. A health education intervention as the control condition in the CTN-0037 STRIDE multi-site exercise trial: rationale and description. Ment Health Phys Act. 2014;7(1):37–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Schwartz C, Chesney M, Irvine M, Keefe F. The control group dilemma in clinical research: applications for psychosocial and behavioral medicine trials. Psychosom Med. 1997;59:362–71.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Solomon P, Cavanaugh M, Draine J. Randomized controlled trials: design and implementation for community-based psychosocial interventions. Oxford: Oxford University Press, Inc; 2009.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  12. Kinser P, Robins J. Control group design: enhancing rigor in research of mind-body therapies for depression. Evid BasedComplement Alternat Med. 2013;2013:140467.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Vickers A, de Craen A. Why use placebos in clinical trials? A narrative review of the methodological literature. J Clin Epidemiol. 2000;53:157–61.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Engel R, Schutt R. Fundamentals of social work research. 2nd ed. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications; 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Wiersma W. Research methods in education. 7th ed. Boston: Allyn and Bacon; 2000.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Petersen S, Zoffmann V, Kjaergaard J, Steensballe L, Greisen G. Disappointment and adherence among parents of newborns allocated to the control group: a qualitative study of a randomized clinical trial. Trials. 2014;15:126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Freeland K. Demanding attention: reconsidering the role of attention control groups in behavioral intervention research. Psychosom Med. 2013;75:100–2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Kazdin A, Bass D. Power to detect differences between alternative treatments in comparitive psychotherapy outcome research. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1989;57:138–47.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Freedland K, Mohr D, Davidson K, Schwartz J. Usual and unusual care: existing practice control groups in randomized controlled trials of behavioral interventions. Psychosom Med. 2011;73:323–35.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Au D, Castro M, Krishnan J. Selection of controls in clinical trials. Proc Am Thorac Soc. 2007;4:567–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. European Medicines Agency: ICH Topic E 10, Choice of control group in clinical trials; Step 5; Note for guidance on choice of control group in clinical trials (CPMP/ICH/364/96). In. London; 2001. http://www.ema.europa.eu/docs/en_GB/document_library/Scientific_guideline/2009/09/WC500002925.pdf.

  22. Townsend MS, Johns M, Shilts MK, Farfan-Ramirez L. Evaluation of a USDA nutrition education program for low-income youth. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2006;38(1):30–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Cuijpers P, Van Straten A, Warmerdam L, Smits N. Characteristics of effective psychological treatments of depression: a meta regression analysis. Psychother Res. 2009;18:225–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Karlsson P, Bergmark A. Compared with what? An analysis of control-group types in Cochrane and Campbell reviews of psychosocial treatment efficacy with substance abuse disorders. Addiction. 2014;110:420–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. de Bruin M, Viechtbauer W, Hospers H, Schaalma H, Kok G. Standard care quality determines treatment outcomes in control groups of HAART-adherence intervention studies: implications for the interpretation of comparision of intervention effects. Health Psychol. 2009;28:668–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Glasziou P, Meats E, Heheghan C, Shepperd S. What is missing from descriptions of treatment in trials and reviews. BMJ Open. 2008;336:1472–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Glanz K, Sorensen G, Farmer A. The health impact of worksite nutrition and cholesterol intervention programs. Am J Health Promot. 1995;10:453–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Glaziou P. What is missing from descriptions of treatment in trials and reviews. BMJ Open. 2008;336:1472–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Nutrition education systematic review project: methodology. https://www.cnpp.usda.gov/nutrition-evidence-library-systematic-review-projects. Accessed 20 Sept 2015.

  30. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman D, Group P. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. 2009;6(7):e1000097.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Armstrong R, Waters E, Jackson N, Oliver S, Popay J, Shepherd J, Petticrew M, Anderson L, Bailie R, Brunton G, et al. Guidelines for systematic reviews of health promotion and public health interventions, version 2. In. London: The Cochrane Collaboration; 2007.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Higgins J, Green S. Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions, version 5.1.0. In. London: The Cochrane Collaboration; 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Oldroyd J, Burns C, Lucas P, Haikerwal A, Waters E. The effectiveness of nutrition interventions on dietary outcomes by relative social disadvantage: a systematic review. J Epidemiol Comm Health. 2008;62(7):573–9.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Ajie WN, Chapman-Novakofski KM. Impact of computer-mediated, obesity-related nutrition education interventions for adolescents: a systematic review. J Adolesc Health. 2014;54(6):631–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Anderson LM, Quinn TA, Glanz K, Ramirez G, Kahwati LC, Johnson DB, Buchanan LR, Archer WR, Chattopadhyay S, Kalra GP, et al. The effectiveness of worksite nutrition and physical activity interventions for controlling employee overweight and obesity: a systematic review. Am J Prev Med. 2009;37(4):340–57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Glenton C, Underland V, Kho M, Pennick V, Oxman A. Summaries of findings, descriptions of interventions, and information about adverse effects would make reviews more informative. J Clin Epidemiol. 2006;59:770–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Bensley RJ, Anderson JV, Brusk JJ, Mercer N, Rivas J. Impact of internet vs traditional special supplemental nutrition program for women, infants, and children nutrition education on fruit and vegetable intake. J Am Diet Assoc. 2011;111(5):749–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Herbert PC, Lohrmann DK, Seo DC, Stright AD, Kolbe LJ. Effectiveness of the energize elementary school program to improve diet and exercise. J School Health. 2013;83(11):780–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Healy N, Joram E, Matvienko O, Woolf S, Knesting K. Impact of an intuitive eating education program on high school Students' eating attitudes. Health Educ. 2015;115:214–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Wall DE, Least C, Gromis J, Lohse B. Nutrition education intervention improves vegetable-related attitude, self-efficacy, preference, and knowledge of fourth-grade students. J School Health. 2012;82(1):37–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Pobocik RS, Haar CM, Dawson EE, Coleman P, Bakies K. Curriculum for junior high school students: dairy food consumption self-efficacy. J Fam Consumer Sci. 2009;101(4):20–6.

    Google Scholar 

  42. McClelland JW, Jayaratne KSU, Bird CL. Nutrition education brings behavior and knowledge change in limited-resource older adults. J Extension. 2013;51(2):Article 2FEA1.

  43. McCaughtry N, Fahlman M, Martin JJ, Shen B. Influences of constructivist-oriented nutrition education on urban middle school Students' nutrition knowledge, self-efficacy, and behaviors. Am J Health Educ. 2011;42(5):276–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Hekler EB, Gardner CD, Robinson TN. Effects of a college course about food and society on students' eating behaviors. Am J Prev Med. 2010;38(5):543–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Devine CM, Farrell TJ, Hartman R. Sisters in health: experiential program emphasizing social interaction increases fruit and vegetable intake among low-income adults. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2005;37(5):265–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Ievers-Landis CE, Burant C, Drotar D, Morgan L, Trapl ES, Colabianchi N, Kwoh K. A randomized controlled trial for the primary prevention of osteoporosis among preadolescent girl scouts: 1-year outcomes of a behavioral program. J Pediatric Psychol. 2005;30(2):155–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Hopper CA, Munoz KD, Gruber MB, Nguyen KP. The effects of a family fitness program on the physical activity and nutrition behaviors of third-grade children. Res Q Exerc Sport. 2005;76(2):130–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Nitzke S, Kritsch K, Boeckner L, Greene G, Hoerr S, Horacek T, Kattelmann K, Lohse B, Oakland M, Phillips B, et al. A stage-tailored multi-model intervention increases fruit and vegetable intakes of low-income young adults. Am J Health Promot. 2007;22:6–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Cook RF, Billings DW, Hersch RK, Back AS, Hendrickson A. A field test of web-based workplace health promotion program to improve dietary practices, reduce stress, and increase physical activity: randomized controlled trial. J Med Internet Res. 2007;9(2):1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Clifford D, Anderson J, Auld G, Champ J. Good grubbin': impact of a TV cooking show for college students living off campus. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2009;41(3):194–200.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. McCarthy WJ, Yancey AK, Harrison GG, Leslie J, Siegel JM. Fighting cancer with fitness: dietary outcomes of a randomized, controlled lifestyle change intervention in healthy African-american women. Prev Med. 2007;44(3):246–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Mitchell RE, Ash SL, McClelland JW. Nutrition education among low-income older adults: a randomized intervention trial in congregate nutrition sites. Health Educ Behav. 2006;33(3):374–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Resnicow K, Davis R, Zhang N, Tolsma D, Alexander G, Wiese C, Cross WE Jr, Anderson JP, Calvi J, Strecher V. Tailoring a fruit and vegetable intervention on ethnic identity: results of a randomized study. Health Psychol. 2009;28(4):394–403.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Gans K, Risica P, Dulin-Keita A, Mello J, Dawood M, Strolla L, Harel O. Innovative video tailoring for dietary change: final results of the good for you! Cluster randomized trial. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2015;12:130.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Dzewaltowski DA, Rosenkranz RR, Geller KS, Coleman KJ, Welk GJ, Hastmann TJ, Milliken GA. HOP'N after-school project: an obesity prevention randomized controlled trial. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2010;7:90.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Greene GW, Fey-Yensan N, Padula C, Rossi SR, Rossi JS, Clark PG. Change in fruit and vegetable intake over 24 months in older adults: results of the SENIOR project intervention. Gerontologist. 2008;48(3):378–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Wolf RL, Lepore SJ, Vandergrift JL, Basch CE, Yaroch AL. Tailored telephone education to promote awareness and adoption of fruit and vegetable recommendations among urban and mostly immigrant black men: a randomized controlled trial. Prev Med. 2009;48(1):32–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Gans KM, Risica PM, Strolla LO, Fournier L, Kirtania U, Upegui D, Zhao J, George T, Acharyya S. Effectiveness of different methods for delivering tailored nutrition education to low income, ethnically diverse adults. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2009;6:24.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Hughes SL, Seymour RB, Campbell RT, Shaw JW, Fabiyi C, Sokas R. Comparison of two health-promotion programs for older workers. Am J Public Health. 2011;101(5):883–90.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Glanz K, Hersey J, Cates S, Muth M, Creel D, Nicholls J, Fulgoni V, Zaripeh S. Effect of a nutrient rich foods consumer education program: results from the nutrition advice study. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2012;112:56–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Ratcliffe MM, Merrigan KA, Rogers BL, Goldberg JP. The effects of school garden experiences on middle school-aged students’ knowledge, attitudes, and behaviors associated with vegetable consumption. Health Promot Pract. 2011;12(1):36–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Dollahite JS, Pijai EI, Scott-Pierce M, Parker C, Trochim W. A randomized controlled trial of a community-based nutrition education program for low-income parents. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2014;46(2):102–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Alexander GL, McClure JB, Calvi JH, Divine GW, Stopponi MA, Rolnick SJ, Heimendinger J, Tolsma DD, Resnicow K, Campbell MK, et al. A randomized clinical trial evaluating online interventions to improve fruit and vegetable consumption. Am J Public Health. 2010;100(2):319–26.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. Elder JP, Ayala GX, Slymen DJ, Arredondo EM, Campbell NR. Evaluating psychosocial and behavioral mechanisms of change in a tailored communication intervention. Health Educ Behav. 2009;36(2):366–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Franko DL, Cousineau TM, Trant M, Green TC, Rancourt D, Thompson D, Ainscough J, Mintz LB, Ciccazzo M. Motivation, self-efficacy, physical activity and nutrition in college students: randomized controlled trial of an internet-based education program. Prev Med. 2008;47(4):369–77.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. Kattelmann KK, Byrd-Bredbenner C, White AA, Greene G, Hoerr S, Kidd T, Colby S, Horacek TM, Phillips BW, Koenings MM, et al. The effects of young adults eating and active for health (YEAH): a theory-based web-delivered intervention. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2014;46:S28–42.

    Google Scholar 

  67. Backman D, Scruggs V, Atiedu AA, Bowie S, Bye L, Dennis A, Hall M, Ossa A, Wertlieb S, Foerster SB. Using a toolbox of tailored educational lessons to improve fruit, vegetable, and physical activity behaviors among African american women in California. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2011;43(4):S75–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Wilcox S, Parrott A, Baruth M, Laken M, Condrasky M, Saunders R, Dowda M, Evans R, Addy C, Warren TY, et al. The faith, activity, and nutrition program: a randomized controlled trial in African-american churches. Am J Prev Med. 2013;44(2):122–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Alaimo K, Carlson J, Pfeiffer K, Eisenmann J, Paek H, Bets H, Thompson T, Wen Y, Norman G. Project FIT: a school, community and social marketing intervention improves healthy eating among low-income elementary school children. J Commun Health. 2015;40:815–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Katz DL, Katz CS, Treu JA, Reynolds J, Njike V, Walker J, Smith E, Michael J. Teaching healthful food choices to elementary school students and their parents: the nutrition detectives[TM] program. J School Health. 2011;81(1):21–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Powers AR, Struempler BJ, Guarino A, Parmer SM. Effects of a nutrition education program on the dietary behavior and nutrition knowledge of second-grade and third-grade students. J School Health. 2005;75(4):129.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Kemirembe OMK, Radhakrishna RB, Gurgevich E, Yoder EP, Ingram PD. An evaluation of nutrition education program for low-income youth. J Extension. 2011;49(3):3.

    Google Scholar 

  73. Bogart LM, Cowgill BO, Elliott MN, Klein DJ, Hawes-Dawson J, Uyeda K, Elijah J, Binkle DG, Schuster MA. A randomized controlled trial of students for nutrition and eXercise: a community-based participatory research study. J Adolesc Health. 2014;55(3):415–22.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  74. United States Department of Health and Human Services, Food and Drug Administration, Center for Drug Evaluation and Research, Center for Biologics Evaluation and Research: Guidance for Industry. E 10 choice of control group and related issues in clinical trials. In. Washington, DC; 2001. https://www.fda.gov/downloads/drugs/guidancecomplianceregulatoryinformation/guidances/ucm073139.pdf.

  75. Castongay L. Controlling is not enough: the importance of measuring the process and specific effectiveness of psychotherapy treatment and control conditions. Ethics Behav. 2002;12:31–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Ferri M, Amato L, Davoli M. Alcoholics anonymous and other 12-step programmes for alcohol dependence. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2006;3:CD005032.

    Google Scholar 

  77. Moncher F, Prinz R. Treatment fidelity in outcome studies. Clin Psychol Rev. 1991;11:247–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  78. Gearing R, El-Bassel N, Ghesquiere A, Baldwin S, Gillies J, Ngeow E. Major ingredients of fidelity: a review and scientific guide to improving quality of intervention research implemention. Clin Psychol Rev. 2011;31:79–88.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Schutt R. Investigating the social world. The process and practices of research. 7th ed. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  80. CONSORT Transparent Reporting of Trials, CONSORT 2010 Flow Diagram http://www.consort-statement.org/consort-statement/flow-diagram. Accessed 20 Augt 2015.

  81. Mikkelsen M, Husby S, Skov L, Perez-Cueto J. A systematic review of types of health eating interventions in preschools. Nutr J. 2014;13:56.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  82. Bond M, Wyatt K, Lloyd J, Welch K, Taylor R. Systematic review of the effectiveness and cost-effectiveness of weight management schemes for the under fives: a short report. Health Technol Assessment. 2009;13(61):1–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  83. Waters L, Reeves M, Fjeldsoe B, Eakin E. Control group improvements in physical activity intervetnion trials and possible explanatory factors: a systematic review. J Phys Act Health. 2012;9:884–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. van Sluijs E, van Poppel M, Twisk J, van Mechelen W. Physical activity measurements affected participants' behavior in a randomized controlled trial. J Clin Epidemiol. 2006;59:404–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Jensen P, Weersing R, Hoagwood K, Goldman E. What is the evidence for evidence-based treatments? A hard look at our soft underbelly. Ment Health Serv Res. 2005;7:53–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Montgomery P, Grant S, Hopewell S, Macdonald G, Moher D, Michie S, Mayo-Wilson E. Protocol for CONSORT-SPI: an extension for social and psychological interventions. Implementation Sci. 2013;8:99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  87. Greaves C, Gillison F, Stathi A, Bennett P, Reddy P, Dunbar J, Perry R, Messom D, Chandler R, Francis M, et al. Waste the waist: a pilot randomized controlled trial of a primary care based interventino to support lifestyle change in people with igh cardiovascular disease. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2015;12:1.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  88. Mayo-Wilson E. Reporting implementation in randomized trials.: proposed additions to the consolidated standars of reporting trials statement. Am J Public Health. 2007;97:630–3.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  89. Chalmers I, Altman D. How can medical journals help prevent poor medical research? Some opportunities presented by electronic publishing. Lancet. 1999;353:490–3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Arean P, Alvidrez J. Ethical considerations in psychotherapy effectiveness research: choosing the comparison group. Ethics Behav. 2002;12:63–73.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Pagoto SL, McDermott M, Reed G, Greenland P, Mazor K, Ockene J, Whited M, Schneider K, Appelhans B, Leung K, et al. Can attention control conditions have detrimental effects in behavioral medicine randomized trials? Psychosom Med. 2013;75:137–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Waters E, de Silva-Sanigorski A, Burford B, Brown T, Cambell K, Gao Y, Armstrong R, Prosser L, Summerbell C. Interventions for preventing obesity in children. Cochrane Database Syst Reviews. 2011;12:CD001871.

    Google Scholar 

  93. Parshuram C, Kavanaugh B. Positive clinical trails. Understand the control group before implementing the result. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2004;170:223–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  94. Boutron I, Moher D, Altman D, Schulz K, Ravaud P. Extending the CONSORT statement to randomized trials of nonpharmacologic treatment: explanation and elaboration. Ann Intern Med. 2006;146:295–309.

    Google Scholar 

  95. Tolsgaard M, Ku C, Woods N, Kulasegaram K, Brydges R, Ringsted C. Quality of randomised controlled trials in medical education between 2012 and 2013: a systematic review. BMJ Open. 2014;4:e005155.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  96. Keihner AJ, Meigs R, Sugerman S, Backman D, Garbolino T, Mitchell P. The power play! Campaign's School Idea & Resource Kits improve determinants of fruit and vegetable intake and physical activity among fourth- and fifth-grade children. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2011;43(4 Suppl 2):S122–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  97. Roofe NL. Improving Families' nutrition knowledge through service learning. J Allied Health. 2011;40(4):194–8.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  98. McCarthy E, Wolff C, Bianco-Simeral S, Crozier J, Goto K. The effects of a school-based nutrition intervention on fruit and vegetable preferences, self-efficacy, and consumption among low-income, Hispanic and white middle-school students. J Child Nutr Manage. 2012;36(2):2.

    Google Scholar 

  99. Eicher-Miller HA, Mason AC, Abbott AR, McCabe GP, Boushey CJ. The effect of food stamp nutrition education on the food insecurity of low-income women participants. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2009;41(3):161–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  100. Madsen K, Linchey J, Gerstein D, Ross M, Myers E, Brown K, Crawford P. Energy balance 4 kids with play: results from a two-year cluster-randomized trial. Child Obes. 2015;11:375–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

This study is funded by the United States Department of Agriculture, National Institute of Food and Agriculture, Grant Number 2011–68001–30,170.

Availability of data and materials

Not applicable.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

All authors were involved in the study design and contributed to the development and implementation of the study. CBB, FW, KS, VQ, JMB conducted the analysis and interpretation of the data. All authors were involved in drafting manuscript components and contributed to editing and reviewing the final manuscript. All authors have read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Virginia Quick.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Additional file

Additional file 1: Table S5.

Factors extracted in systematic review of articles. (DOCX 37 kb)

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Byrd-Bredbenner, C., Wu, F., Spaccarotella, K. et al. Systematic review of control groups in nutrition education intervention research. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 14, 91 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12966-017-0546-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12966-017-0546-3

Keywords