Skip to main content

Table 1 An illustrative list of constructs applicable to ASR in childhood and their measurement

From: “Food” and “non-food” self-regulation in childhood: a review and reciprocal analysis

Construct

First author, publication year

Sample description/age focus for reviews

Construct Measurement

Study design

Results/selected findings

Bottom up (approach)

 Food responsiveness

Carnell (2016) [97]

4–5 year old UK children and mothers, mainly White British

CEBQ

Cross-sectional, with preloading and observed lunch intake plus parent reported eating behaviors

Higher food responsiveness was linked to greater total food intake

Cross (2014) [98]

Low income African American and Hispanic parents and their preschool children aged 4 years

CEBQ

Cross-sectional survey of parent rated eating behaviors and parent feeding practices

Food responsiveness related to mothers’ restrictive feeding practices

 Reward sensitivity/response to food and food cues

Adise (2019) [99]

7–11 year old US children and families, mainly White, middle income

fMRI

Cross-sectional using neuro imaging assessment of brain responses to food and money rewards

Higher food responsiveness linked to decreased brain responses to winning food rewards. Regions associated with reward, cognitive control and emotion may play a role in the brain’s responses to food

Yokum (2019) [100]

US adolescents 14–17 years, 77.7% European American

fMRI

Longitudinal design measuring neural activity for gained weight versus weight stable groups

Suggest that initial hyper-responsivity to palatable high-fat food tastes could be related to future weight gain

Shapiro (2019) [101]

From a pre-birth longitudinal cohort of US children, ethnically diverse. Tested at 4–6 years of age

fMRI

Cross-sectional with laboratory-based measurement of Eating in absence of hunger (EAH) and a brain scan

EAH was associated with activity in a major reward network, and reduced connectivity between brain regions associated with reward and those associated with response inhibition

 Enjoyment of food

Carnell (2016) [97]

4–5 year old UK children and mothers, mainly White British

CEBQ

Cross-sectional, with preloading and observed lunch intake plus parent reported eating behaviors

Higher enjoyment of food was linked to greater total food intake

Cross (2014) [98]

Low income African American and Hispanic parents and their preschool children aged 4 years

CEBQ

Cross-sectional survey of parent rated eating behaviors and parent feeding practices

Higher enjoyment of food was linked to more restrictive feeding practices in African American families

 Hedonic/reward aspects of food and hunger

Alonso-Alonso (2015) [102]

Not age-based

Review

Examined the neuroscience of food reward

Discussed homeostatic and non-homeostatic (related to the brain’s reward system) influences on the regulation of food intake

Lowe (2007) [103]

Not age-based

Review

Examined hedonic hunger as a new eating motive

Proposed a distinction between homeostatic and hedonic eating

 Subliminal reward signals

de Araujo (2020) [104]

Not age-based

Review

Examined human and animal research about processes associated with food reward

Proposed a two-path model of food reward that included subliminal reward signals and conscious liking

 Reward neurocircuitry

Reichelt (2015) [105]

Not age-based

Review

Examined neurocircuitry associated with the reinforcing value of foods and inhibitory control

Set out a model of food cue effects on homeostatic appetite signals and reward neurocircuitry

 Emotional eating/over-eating

Lumeng (2014) [106]

Low-income (Head Start) US children aged 3–4 years and parents

CEBQ

Cross-sectional using parent questionnaires plus child weight and cortisol measures

Family stress was linked to overweight, with this mediated by emotional eating in boys

 External eating

Jahnke (2008) [107]

German mothers of preschool children aged 3–6 years. Diverse SES

DEBQ

Cross-sectional using parent-questionnaires

Overweight children scored higher on external eating

 Consumption of problematic foods

Jahnke (2008) [107]

German mothers of preschool children aged 3–6 years, diverse SES

Parent reports of child food consumption

Cross-sectional using parent questionnaires

Parent ratings showed that children with higher weight status ate significant less problematic food

 Healthy food preferences

Anzman-Frasca (2018) [108]

Prenatal to early childhood

Review

Examined evidence about promoting healthy food preferences

Early exposure to healthy foods can support subsequent acceptance of these foods

Russell (2016) [109]

Diverse sample of Australian preschool children aged 3–5 years and parents

Parent reports of food preferences

Cross-sectional, with measures of parent-reported child appetitive traits (CEBQ) and food preferences

Healthy food preferences were related to enjoyment of food, satiety responsiveness and fussiness

 Eating in the absence of hunger (EAH)

Leung (2014) [58]

Low-income (Head Start) US preschool children and their caregivers, diverse in race and ethnicity

Observed EAH using the free access protocol

Cross-sectional with measures of parent-reports of temperament and obesogenic eating behaviors plus observed EAH

Higher temperamental surgency, but not effortful control, was related to more EAH

 Impulsivity

Bennett (2016) [110]

UK parents (mainly tertiary educated) and their children aged 2–4 years

Parent ratings on ECBQ, and child impulsivity, plus laboratory assessments of child impulsivity

Cross-sectional, using parent questionnaires and laboratory measures

Girls high in trait-like impulsivity and boys high in motor impulsivity could be more prone to display food approach behaviors associated with weight gain when parents monitor their intake less.

 Disinhibited eating

Shapiro (2019) [101]

From a pre-birth longitudinal cohort of US children, ethnically diverse. Tested at 4–6 years of age

Disinhibited eating measured using the EAH free access protocol

Cross-sectional with laboratory measurement of EAH and a brain scan

Provided new evidence of the neuronal correlates of disinhibited eating in young children

Russell (2018) [29]

Childhood

Review

Narrative review of development of appetitive traits using insights from research and theory in developmental science

Outlined a biopsychosocial model of the development of appetitive traits, including disinhibited eating in childhood

 Eating rate

Carnell (2007) [111]

UK children 4–5 years of age and parents (mainly mothers, White British and affluent)

Observed eating rate

Cross-sectional with observed eating behaviors plus parent-completed CEBQ

Faster eating was linked to higher food responsiveness and enjoyment of food. Slower eating was linked to higher satiety responsiveness

Bottom up (avoidance)

 Food neophobia/picky eating

Russell (2018) [29]

Childhood

Review

Narrative review of development of appetitive traits using insights from research and theory in developmental science

Outlined a biopsychosocial model of the development of appetitive traits, including food neophobia in childhood

Cole (2017) [112]

Children less than 30 months of age

Review

Examined correlates of picky eating and food neophobia at different levels, for example, genetic, child, family, community

Highlighted the importance of investigating parent-child dyads and bidirectional feeding patterns

Russell (2008) [113]

Population-based sample of Australian children 2–5 years and parents

CFNS

Cross-sectional, with measures of parent-reported food neophobia and food preferences

Food neophobia was negatively correlated with liking for all foods in the healthy food group of Australian Healthy Eating Guide

Lumeng (2018) [114]

Low income US children and mothers. Entered study at 21 or 27 months of age.

CEBQ, BAMBI

Cross-lagged cohort questionnaire study at 21, 27 and 33 months of age

Concurrent association were found between picky eating and pressuring feeding, but no prospective associations

 Food fussiness

Gregory (2010) [115]

Australian mothers of children 2–4 years mostly tertiary educated and Australian born

CEBQ

Cross-sectional using parent questionnaires about child eating behaviors, parent feeding, and concerns about child weight

Food fussiness predicted maternal pressure to eat, partially mediated by concern about child underweight

 Food avoidance

Powell (2011) [116]

UK mothers of children 3–6 years, mostly White British

CEBQ

Cross-sectional with parent reports of parent feeding behaviors and child food avoidance

Maternal feeding practices significantly predicted child food avoidance

 Emotional undereating

Bjorklund (2018) [117]

Representative community sample of Norwegian children 6–10 years and parents

CEBQ

Longitudinal with measures of child and contextual predictors of change in emotional over- and undereating

Lower family functioning at age 6 predicted emotional undereating at age 10

Herle (2018) [118]

Subsample from Twins Early Development study at age 4 years, mainly White British

CEBQ

Cross-sectional with measures of genetic and environmental factors contributing to emotional over-and undereating

Genetic contributions to emotional undereating were not significant. Shared environmental factors explained 77% of the variance

 Slowness in eating

Llewellyn (2010) [98]

Population-based sample of infant twins from England and Wales

BEBQ

Cross sectional heritability analysis of scales from BEBQ

Heritability was high for slowness in eating

Top down

 Delay-of-gratification

Lelakowski (2019) [72]

Diverse US sample of mothers, children aged 24–30 months

Snack delay task

Longitudinal, with measures of child temperament, parent feeding and child BMI

Impulsivity but not inhibitory control (snack delay task) was related to BMI

Kidd (2013) [119]

US children aged 3–5 years

Marshmallow wait task

Cross-sectional with measures of children’s wait time and beliefs about environmental reliability

Wait time reflected differences in self-control and beliefs about the stability of the world

 Reward/delay discounting

Bennett (2019) [120]

UK children aged 7–11 years and parents, mainly White middle class

Delay discounting task as a measure of impulsivity

Cross-sectional with measures of child impulsivity, adiposity, intake during a snack, and eating behaviors

Poorer performance on delay discounting was associated with greater snack intake

 EC inhibitory control

Rollins (2014) [121]

US children aged 3–7 years and parents mainly White, middle to high income

CBQ

Short-term longitudinal with measures of restrictive feeding practices, intake of restricted food and child weight

Children with lower inhibitory control and higher approach showed greater increase in intake in association with experience of parental restriction

Tan (2011) [122]

US parents with children 3–9 years

CBQ

Cross-sectional with measures of child self-regulation in eating, inhibitory control and parents’ feeding behavior

Self-regulation in eating was positively correlated with inhibitory control

 EF inhibitory control

Fogel (2019) [123]

Children from an Asian cohort aged 6 years

Stop signal task as measure of inhibitory control

Cross-sectional with measures of child inhibitory control, eating behavior and adiposity

Lower inhibitory control was related to selecting larger food portion, multiple food servings and faster eating rates

Shapiro (2019) [124]

From a pre-birth longitudinal cohort of US children, ethnically diverse. Tested at 4–6 years of age

Flanker task as measure of inhibitory control

Cross-sectional with measures of biomarkers of poor metabolic health and performance on cognitive tasks

Greater blood biomarkers of poor metabolic health were related to lower inhibitory control

Others/both top-down and bottom-up

 Homeostatic and hedonic systems cross-talk

Higgs (2017) [125]

Not age-based

Review

Examined evidence about the integration of metabolic, reward and cognitive processes in appetite control

Favors a framework that emphasizes cross-talk between the neurochemical substrates of hedonic and homeostatic systems

Berthoud (2017) [126]

Not age-based

Review

Examined hedonic and homeostatic controls in the regulation of body weight

Presents neural models of the interaction between homeostatic and hedonic controls

 Interoception

Keller (2018) [127]

Children

Review

Examined the role of the brain in children’s food choice and eating behavior, including brain regions associated with interoception

Noted findings suggesting a reduced awareness of internal homeostatic cues among individuals prone to obesity

 Alliesthesia

Higgs (2017) [125]

Not age-based

Review

Examined evidence about the integration of metabolic, reward and cognitive processes in appetite control

Discussed alliesthesia: food is more liked when hungry, less so when eating when full. Noted associations with decreases in reward-related brain activations

Berridge (2010) [128]

Not age-based

Review

Examined brain mechanisms associated with obesity or eating disorders, including alliesthesia

Suggested possible brain-based mechanisms for hunger increasing “liking” and “wanting” food

 Caloric compensation

Carnell (2007) [111]

UK children 4–5 years of age and parents, (mainly mothers, White British and affluent)

Observed using preload protocol

Cross-sectional with measures of children’s ability to regulate intake depending on the caloric content of a preload plus parent-completed CEBQ

Higher satiety responsiveness (CEBQ) was associated with better average caloric compensation

 Compensation for energy density

Brugaileres (2019) [129]

French infants at 11 and 15 months of age and mothers

Observed using preload protocol

Short-term longitudinal with measures of changes in adjustment of intake to energy density

At both ages, infants undercompensated for the energy of the preload. Compensation ability decreased from 11 to 15 months. The greater the decrease, the higher weight status at 2 years of age

 

Johnson (2000) [28]

High SES US children 4–5 years of age and parents

Preload protocol

Short-term longitudinal intervention to help children recognize cues of satiety and hunger to compensate for energy density

Large individual differences in self-regulation at baseline. The intervention improved children’s self-regulation

 Compensation across meals and over days

Leahy (2008) [130]

US children 3–5 years of age. Parents mostly White with a university degree

Varied energy density of prepared meals

Short-term longitudinal with measures of intake in response to differences in energy density over 2 days using a cross-over design

A decrease in energy density led to a decrease in energy intake; children did not compensate in their energy intake (calories) according to the energy density of the meals

 Food choice/processed food effects

Small (2019) [131]

Not age-based

Review

Examined two systems driving food choice: metabolic signals about nutritional content, and conscious perceptions e.g., about flavor, caloric content, healthfulness

Argues there is evidence that nutritional signals about processed food are not accurately conveyed to the brain

 Food “liking” and “wanting”

Keller (2018) [127]

Children

Review

Examined the role of the brain in children’s food choice and eating behavior, including the neural drivers of food “liking” and “wanting”

Summarizes evidence about the neural drivers of affective response to food (“liking”) and the incentive salience of food (“wanting”)

Berridge (2016) [132]

Not age-based

Review

Examined brain mechanisms associated with “wanting” a reward (including food) and “liking” the same reward

Addiction could be associated with excessive amplification of “wanting”, especially triggered by cues about anticipated rewards and pleasure. Heightened dopamine reactivity such as stress and emotions could increase “wanting”

 Satiety responsiveness

Carnell (2016) [97]

4–5 year old UK children and mothers, mainly White British

CEBQ

Cross-sectional, with preloading and observed lunch intake plus parent-reported eating behaviors

Higher satiety responsiveness was linked to lower total food intake

Cross (2014) [98]

Low income African American and Hispanic parents and their preschool children

CEBQ

Cross-sectional using measures of parent-rated child eating behaviors and parent-reported feeding practices

Higher satiety responsiveness was associated with greater pressure to eat in African American families

 Satiation and satiety

Blundell (2010) [133]

Not age-based

Review

Examined specific measures of satiation, satiety, hunger and food consumption, including “liking” and “wanting”

Sets out a model of the impact of foods on satiation and satiety. Discussed approaches to the measurement of satiation and satiety

Bellisle (2012) [134]

Not age-based

Review

Examines the satiating power of foods with sweeteners. Included “liking” and “wanting” and their role

Highlighted methodological challenges in measuring satiation and satiety

  1. CEBQ Children’s Eating Behaviour Questionnaire, BEBQ Baby Eating Behaviour Questionnaire, TMCQ Temperament in Middle Childhood Questionnaire, ECBQ Early Childhood Behaviour Questionnaire, CFNS Child Food Neophobia Scale, BAMBI Brief Autism Mealtime Behaviour Inventory, CBQ Children’s Behavior Questionnaire, EF Executive Function, EC Effortful Control