Skip to main content

Impact of feedback generation and presentation on self-monitoring behaviors, dietary intake, physical activity, and weight: a systematic review and meta-analysis

A Correction to this article was published on 23 February 2024

This article has been updated

Abstract

Self-monitoring of dietary intake, physical activity, and weight is a key strategy in behavioral interventions, and some interventions provide self-monitoring feedback to facilitate goal setting and promote engagement. This systematic review aimed to evaluate whether feedback increases intervention effectiveness, and which forms of feedback presentation (e.g., personalized vs. not personalized) and generation (i.e., human vs. algorithm-generated) are most effective. To achieve this aim, 5 electronic databases (PubMed/MEDLINE, Web of Science, CINAHL, PsycINFO, and Google Scholar) were searched in April 2022 and yielded 694 unique records, out of which 24 articles reporting on 19 studies were included (with a total of 3261 participants). Two reviewers independently screened titles and abstracts and then full texts and categorized articles as eligible or excluded according to the pre-registered criteria (i.e., availability of full text, peer reviewed manuscript in English; adult participants in a randomized controlled trial that included both self-monitoring and feedback; comparisons of different forms of feedback or comparisons of feedback vs. no feedback; primary outcomes of diet, physical activity, self-monitoring behavior, and/or weight). All included studies were assessed for methodological quality independently by two reviewers using the revised Cochrane risk-of-bias tool for randomized studies (version 2). Ten studies compared feedback to no feedback, 5 compared human- vs. algorithm-generated feedback, and the remaining 4 studies compared formats of feedback presentation (e.g., frequency, richness). A random effects meta-analysis indicated that physical activity interventions with feedback provision were more effective than physical activity interventions without feedback (d=0.29, 95% CI [0.16;0.43]). No meta-analysis could be conducted for other comparisons due to heterogeneity of study designs and outcomes. There were mixed results regarding which form of feedback generation and presentation is superior. Limitations of the evidence included in this review were: lack of details about feedback provided, the brevity of most interventions, the exclusion of studies that did not isolate feedback when testing intervention packages, and the high risk of bias in many studies. This systematic review underlines the importance of including feedback in behavioral interventions; however, more research is needed to identify most effective forms of feedback generation and presentation to maximize intervention effectiveness.

Trial registration (PROSPERO)

CRD42022316206.

Background

Overweight and obesity remains a substantial public health challenge worldwide and particularly in higher income countries [1]. Behavioral weight management programs, which use behavioral skills training to help individuals make changes in their eating and activity habits, remain the first-line strategy for overweight and obesity management [2]. Self-monitoring of dietary intake, physical activity, and weight plays a key role in these programs [3, 4], and greater adherence to self-monitoring has been demonstrated to be the best predictor of weight loss success [5,6,7,8]. In particular, early and consistent engagement in self-monitoring is important for weight management success [9, 10]. In order to both support greater adherence to self-monitoring and to help individuals interpret the data collected from self-monitoring to set effective behavioral goals, participants in weight management programs are typically provided feedback based on their self-monitoring [11].

Within Social Cognitive Theory, provision of feedback is postulated to provide positive reinforcement for successful goal attainment, insight into potential barriers and challenges, and support for problem-solving and effective development of future goals [12, 13]. Moreover, Supportive Accountability Theory [14, 15] posits that interventionist support is essential for promoting engagement with health behavior change interventions, which often takes the form of feedback. However, beyond these basic theoretical principles, surprisingly little empirical evidence exists to guide the crafting of feedback messages for weight management as well as improvements in dietary intake and physical activity [16].

Feedback has been recognized as a potentially essential ingredient in the behavioral change technique taxonomy [17], and while self-monitoring has been the focus of many systematic reviews/meta-analyses [5, 7, 18, 19], feedback has received less attention [20, 21]. Sherrington and colleagues [20] focused on the personalization of feedback (i.e., individualization of feedback either by a human or an algorithm) in internet-based weight management studies, and they found that personalized feedback may confer approximately a 2 kg benefit over interventions that did not provide personalized feedback. Schembre et al. [21] concentrated on just-in-time feedback in diet and physical activity-focused interventions and was unable to conduct a meta-analysis due to the variability in targeted behaviors, study duration, and feedback types. Thus, it is essential to identify and evaluate feedback types that may optimize this intervention component, given the widespread use of feedback in behavioral interventions for weight management, dietary change and physical activity change, the personnel costs of human-generated feedback [22], as well as the various forms in which feedback may be presented (e.g., positive reinforcement messages vs. areas for change [23]; numerical displays [24, 25] vs. vibrations [26] vs. text [27,28,29]).

The primary aim of the current study was to systematically review and, if possible, meta-analyze self-monitoring interventions that use feedback as a behavior change technique (BCT), to determine the impact of feedback on diet and physical activity behaviors, weight, and self-monitoring behaviors (i.e., diet or physical activity, or weight). The secondary aims were to evaluate aspects of feedback (e.g., how different types of feedback are perceived by participants, how feedback impacts retention, what types of feedback are typically provided, how frequently feedback is provided, the length of feedback) to determine whether there are potentially feedback elements that are associated with superior outcomes.

Methods

The review proposal was submitted to PROSPERO prior to data extraction; it was accepted on April 11, 2022, registration number: CRD42022316206. The search strategy, raw data, and analysis scripts are provided on the Open Science Framework (OSF; https://osf.io/j9duf/).

Search methods for identification of studies

A medical librarian (AD) searched PubMed/MEDLINE, Web of Science, CINAHL, PsycINFO, and Google Scholar. The search was limited to articles published in the English language and published from 1970 through March 2022. Keywords included “self monitor” OR “self monitoring” OR “self monitored” OR “self directed” OR “self evaluate” OR “self regulate” OR “self regulated” OR “self track” OR “self tracking” OR “self weighing” AND obes* OR overweight OR weigh* OR “body mass” OR bmi OR calor* OR diet OR exercise* OR “physical fitness” OR “physical activity” OR walk* OR step OR steps OR pedometer* AND feedback AND behavior* OR behaviour*. Search strategies were modified for each database, utilizing controlled vocabularies (e.g., Medical Subject Headings) as appropriate. Complete search strategies are provided on the OSF website. In addition, searches of reference lists of identified studies and forward citation tracking using Google Scholar was performed by two authors (RAK and LK) to identify further eligible publications.

Screening

All potentially-eligible study records generated from the search strategy were imported into Covidence systematic review software (Veritas Health Innovation, Melbourne, Australia; available at www.covidence.org). Duplicates were removed before all titles and abstracts were screened independently by two authors (RAK and LK), categorizing articles as provisionally eligible or excluded according to the pre-registered eligibility criteria (Table 1).

Table 1 Inclusion and exclusion criteria

Specifically, articles were evaluated on the following criteria (in order) and categorized as excluded on the first criterion where they did not meet eligibility (if applicable): 1) no full text, 2) not published in English, 3) not an empirical peer-reviewed paper, 4) participants were not adults, 5) not a randomized controlled trial, 6) not an intervention targeting diet, physical activity or self-weighing, 7) BCTs did not include both self-monitoring and feedback (of behavior or outcome of behavior), 8) did not compare different forms of feedback or did not compare 2 or more interventions that only differ in whether feedback is provided, and 9) did not include primary outcomes of diet, physical activity, self-monitoring behavior and/or body weight. Conflicts were resolved by discussion. Afterwards, all full texts were screened independently by the same two authors and coded as eligible or excluded. Again, conflicts were resolved by discussion. The flow of study records is documented in the PRISMA diagram (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

PRISMA Flow Chart

Feedback on behavior and outcomes was defined based on the behavior change technique taxonomy [30]. Specifically, feedback on behavior was defined as “Monitor and provide informative or evaluative feedback on performance of the behavior (e.g., form, frequency, duration, intensity); e.g., inform the person of how many steps they walked each day (as recorded on a pedometer) or how many calories they ate each day (based on a food consumption questionnaire).” Feedback on outcomes was defined as “Monitor and provide feedback on the outcome of performance of the behavior; e.g., inform the person of how much weight they have lost following the implementation of a new exercise regime.”

Data extraction and synthesis

Two reviewers (RAK and LK) extracted data into a structured coding form. The data extracted included study characteristics (i.e., target behavior(s), country where the intervention took place, inclusion/exclusion criteria, study conditions, sample size, participant characteristics), intervention characteristics (i.e., theoretical foundation, self-monitoring modality, feedback modality, intervention components, self-monitoring duration), feedback characteristics (i.e., frequency, length, type), outcome characteristics, and study results (i.e., effect size for targeted outcomes, overall study conclusions). We also extracted information about feedback perceptions and retention. All relevant study outcomes were included in the extraction and synthesis.

In addition, a meta-analysis was conducted if at least three studies using similar manipulations and reporting on the same outcome provided data on group means and standard deviations or standard errors that could be used to calculate Cohen’s d [31]. We used metafor 3.8–1 [32] in R Studio 2021.09.2/ R version 4.1.2 to compute random effects models to calculate pooled effect sizes and to adjust for potential publication bias using the trim-and-fill method [33]. Heterogeneity was evaluated using I2 as recommended by Higgins et al. [34]. To account for multiple comparisons (i.e., when the control group was used for more than one comparison), the N of the control group was split, as recommended by Harrer et al. [35]; this was only the case for one study [36].

Risk of bias assessment

All studies that were eligible for inclusion were assessed for methodological quality by two reviewers using the revised Cochrane risk-of-bias tool for randomized studies (version 2) [37]. Studies were evaluated related to 6 types of bias: selection bias, performance bias, detection bias, attrition bias, reporting bias, and other sources of bias. Each type of bias is rated as 1) low risk of bias, 2) some concerns, or 3) high risk of bias. For the overall rating, the category indicating the highest risk of bias for an individual component is used. In addition, Egger’s test was conducted to test for publication bias in the meta-analyses [38, 39].

Results

The literature search yielded 1,396 studies, of which 647 were duplicate citations, 21 were books or chapters and 34 were reviews or meta-analyses, leaving 694 articles to be screened for eligibility. 544 articles were excluded upon title or abstract screening because the study did not meet the inclusion criteria. Thus, 138 full-text articles were assessed for eligibility. After 120 articles that did not meet the inclusion criteria were excluded, there were 18 included publications. An additional 6 studies were identified through forward and backward citation searches. A total of 24 publications reporting on 19 studies were included in the review (see PRISMA diagram, Fig. 1), with a total of 3,261 participants.

Characteristics of included studies

Of the 19 studies, 6 focused on diet [27, 28, 40,41,42,43], 14 focused on physical activity [24,25,26, 36, 41, 43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52], 3 focused on sedentary behaviors [24, 26, 53], and 9 focused on weight management [24, 27,28,29, 40,41,42,43, 51] (with some studies focusing on more than one of these behaviors) (Table 2). Outcomes for the different behaviors, however, varied widely between studies. Dietary behaviors reported comprised energy intake [27, 41, 47, 54], percent carbohydrates, protein, fat, and saturated fat from total daily energy intake, sodium intake, total fiber, added sugar [54], percent saturated, monounsaturated and polyunsaturated fats from daily energy intake [27], daily vegetable and fruit portions, weekly consumption of sweetened beverages and ultraprocessed foods [43], and achievement of diet goals [40]. Regarding physical activity, studies investigated total minutes of physical activity [50], daily minutes of moderate to vigorous physical activity [43, 51], steps [25, 26, 36, 46], walking lengths [47], physical activity energy expenditure [41, 44], metabolic equivalents [24, 48], accelerometer counts [52], activity data [45], time spend standing [26], sedentary time [26, 53], achievement of physical activity goals [50] (see raw data provided on the OSF).

Table 2 Study Characteristics by Comparison Category

The median number of study participants was 80 (range: 17–828). The majority of studies included samples with a mean age between 30 and 50 [27, 28, 40,41,42, 44, 46, 51, 52] who were predominantly female [25, 27,28,29, 36, 40,41,42,43,44, 46, 48, 51, 52] and White [26,27,28,29, 41, 42, 44, 46, 51, 53] (although ethnicity or race was not reported in 7 publications). Studies were conducted in the United States (n = 10, 52.6%; [27,28,29, 41, 42, 46, 47, 51,52,53], Brazil (n = 1, 5.3% [43]), the Netherlands (n = 1, 5.3% [40]), Finland (n = 1, 5.3% [24]), Thailand (n = 1, 5.3% [25]), China (n = 1, 5.3% [45]), and the United Kingdom (n = 4, 21.1% [26, 36, 44, 48]. Study duration ranged from 2 days to 24 months, although most interventions (n = 11) were shorter than 12 weeks. The studies were published between 2005 and 2022.

Theoretical foundation

In total, 11 studies (57.9%) cited a theoretical foundation for the intervention. The most frequently cited theories were general Behavioral Theory (n = 2, 10.6%), Control Theory (n = 2, 10.6%), and Social Cognitive Theory (n = 4, 21.1%).

Retention

Retention rates were calculated by the number of participants who completed the final follow-up data collection visit (varying between studies from 2 days to 24 months). On average, retention was 76.3% (range: 48.0%-100%).

Feedback features of included studies

The 19 studies varied in the content, frequency, and the timing of the feedback, with often limited details provided about the feedback (Table 3). Feedback was often graphical, such as a history of physical activity data, or reduced to numerical indicators for activity units or steps (e.g., [25, 48]). Some studies provided feedback on goal attainment (e.g., [43, 46]) or positive reinforcement (e.g., [29, 40]). Most studies, especially if published recently, provided feedback on digital devices such as PDAs (e.g., [27]), smartphone apps (e.g., [51]), or smartwatches [45]. Frequency of feedback varied, the majority of studies provided feedback continuously (3 studies; [25, 26, 45]), daily (or multiple times daily) (5 studies; [28, 29, 42, 47, 48]), or weekly (3 studies; [36, 41, 53]). Other studies had varied feedback frequency throughout the study (i.e., decreasing frequency) [27], randomization to different frequencies [46], or did not describe the frequency of feedback [24, 40, 43, 51].

Table 3 Feedback Characteristics

Across the studies, 9 compared feedback to no feedback [24, 25, 28, 36, 40, 42, 45, 48, 51, 52] and 5 compared human- versus algorithm-generated feedback [29, 41, 43, 47, 53]. The remaining 4 studies included other types of feedback comparisons, including feedback frequency (daily vs. weekly [27, 46]), richness of feedback (simple vs. visual vs. contextualized [44]), and the behavior on which feedback was provided (upright time vs. sedentary time [26]).

Impact of feedback on diet and physical activity behaviors, weight, and self-monitoring behaviors

A file containing means and standard deviations for all group comparisons can be found on the OSF (https://osf.io/j9duf/).

Impact of feedback provision

Nine studies compared participants who received and did not receive feedback, allowing us to test whether providing feedback had a positive impact on behaviors or weight. Studies yielded mixed results. Six studies reported benefits of feedback such as reaching diet goals [40], self-monitoring diet and exercise more frequently [40], losing more weight [40], and being more physically active [24, 36, 45, 48, 52]. This positive impact, however, was not universally observed; other comparisons did not report an impact of feedback provision on physical activity [25, 52] or weight [24, 28, 42].

Due to the large heterogeneity of studies in terms of feedback provided and outcomes studied (e.g., reporting weight change in various ways), we were only able to conduct a random effects meta-analyses for differences in physical activity based on 9 comparisons reported in 6 studies [25, 36, 45, 48, 51, 52]. The meta-analysis yielded a statistically significant pooled effect size of Cohen’s d = 0.29, 95% CI [0.16;0.43] (test for overall effect: Z = 4.14, p < 0.001; see Fig. 2). Heterogeneity was low (I2 = 9.07, Tau2 = 0.00, H2 = 1.00, df = 9, p = 0.432 [56]). Results were unchanged when using trim-and-fill, indicating no evidence for publication bias (see Fig. 3).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Forest plot for the random effects meta-analysis comparing the impact of providing feedback vs not providing feedback on physical activity behaviors

Fig. 3
figure 3

Funnel plot created using the trim-and-fill method. No studies were filled, indicating that publication bias is unlikely

Impact of human vs. algorithm-generated feedback

Five studies compared the impact of human- and algorithm-generated feedback on behaviors and weight with mixed results. Studies reported significant group differences, including healthier diet composition in participants receiving human-generated feedback [43] and a reduction in sedentary time [53]. Conversely, West et al. [29] reported greater weight loss in participants who received algorithm-generated feedback, compared to participants who received human-generated feedback. Other studies did not report differences between groups for physical activity [41, 43], self-monitoring behaviors [43], or weight loss [41]. Due to the large heterogeneity of studies in terms of feedback provided and outcomes studied, we were unable to conduct any meta-analyses.

Impact of other forms of feedback

Five studies investigated the impact of different forms of feedback on behaviors. Due to the large heterogeneity of studies in terms of feedback provided and outcomes studied, no meta-analysis could be conducted with these studies. The impact of feedback frequency was tested in two studies, including the SMART trial that resulted in several publications (as described below). Kerrigan et al. [46] reported that providing daily feedback increased step counts more than providing weekly feedback. This finding was not mirrored by the publications stemming from the SMART trial related to weight management (i.e., the primary SMART study outcome) [27, 49] or physical activity [27]; however, the SMART trial reported a greater reduction in energy consumed for participants who received daily vs. weekly feedback messages [54] and found greater adherence to self-monitoring if daily feedback messages were provided [55]. Self-monitoring behaviors were then correlated with greater adherence to physical activity goals and weight loss [49, 50].

In a test of another type of feedback, Godino et al. [44] tested whether feedback richness (simple vs. visual vs. contextualized) impacted participants’ physical activity, and found no significant group differences. In addition, Rabbi et al. [47] tested whether personalized feedback (personalized vs. non-personalized feedback, both generated by an algorithm) affected participants’ diet and physical activity. The authors reported that personalized feedback led to increased physical activity, but dietary behaviors were not different between the conditions. Finally, Martin et al. [26] investigated if the behavior on which feedback (i.e., sedentary time vs. upright time) was provided impacted participants’ physical activity. Again, no significant group differences were found.

Impact of feedback on participants’ perception of the intervention and retention

Seven of the included studies [25, 26, 36, 44, 47, 51, 52] reported on participants’ evaluation of the provided feedback. In all seven studies, evaluations were highly positive, with participants reporting that the feedback was motivating [26] and the main reason for using the intervention device [25, 52]. Fanning et al. [51] reported that participants asked for more frequent messages, and Paschali et al. [52] noted that participants in the “no feedback” condition were disappointed that they did not receive any feedback. Feedback thus seems to be an integral component of interventions that participants expect and enjoy. Somewhat unexpectedly, participants even reported that they found the feedback motivating and enjoyable even if it was not related with objectively measured or even perceived changes in behavior (e.g., [26, 52]).

Three of the included studies explicitly compared retention rates between conditions. All three studies [29, 41, 42] did not report differences in retention based on the condition, suggesting that feedback might not prevent attrition.

Risk of bias assessment

We used the Cochrane Risk of Bias 2.0 tool to evaluate all of the studies. All studies were subject to significant risk of bias (see Table 4 for details), with 9 studies having the overall rating of some concern and 10 studies receiving the overall rating of high risk of bias. The high risk of bias largely resulted from lack of pre-registration of the analysis plan.

Table 4 Risk of bias assessment for included studies

Discussion

Feedback is a core component of behavioral change interventions [17]; however, because feedback is rarely the focus of intervention and thus varied systematically, little is known about how feedback should ideally be formulated and presented. The current systematic review aimed to compile the existing evidence about feedback on self-monitoring behaviors, dietary intake, physical activity, and weight. Overall, evidence for the effectiveness of feedback was mixed. There was a significant effect for feedback (vs. no feedback) on physical activity, but this finding was driven by only half of the studies reporting a significant effect for including feedback (compared to no feedback). However, the effect of the presence or absence of feedback for outcomes other than physical activity has rarely been examined and thus we were unable to conduct meta-analyses for these other outcomes.

Despite the popularity of digital interventions which often incorporate algorithm-generated feedback [57, 58], effects of providing human- vs algorithm-generated feedback is understudied. Interestingly, while results of four out of five included studies reported either no difference or human-generated feedback to be superior, findings by West et al. [29] suggest that algorithm-generated feedback may be more effective in certain circumstances. For example, algorithms consistently provide feedback on all of the desired behaviors, which may not happen with a human, and algorithms can provide more immediate feedback, without consideration for holidays, illness, or weekends. In addition, complex algorithms may detect patterns of behavior that may be beyond the capabilities of an interventionist. More research is urgently needed to understand which form of feedback generation are most effective under which circumstances, given that generating feedback automatically may improve the cost-effectiveness and sustainability of behavioral interventions as well as their reach [20].

Available research regarding feedback frequency was especially limited. Two studies [46, 55] focused on the frequency of providing feedback, showing that daily feedback was associated with greater self-monitoring, which was in turn associated with improved behavioral and health outcomes such as physical activity and weight loss. The link between self-monitoring and intervention effectiveness has been previously established; providing feedback frequently (but also not too frequently so that it may annoy users, especially when paired with a notification [59]) may thus be key for intervention effectiveness. More research is needed to confirm these findings also for other behaviors and to determine potential dose–response effects of feedback for the engagement with intervention components.

It is important to note that there are numerous characteristics of within the design of each feedback package (e.g., frequency, behavioral vs. outcome focus, length, personalization, graphical vs. numerical vs. text vs. vibration modality, achievement vs. future behavior change valence). Due to the infrequency of each characteristic of feedback and the lack of systematic manipulation of some of these characteristics, we were not able to evaluate the independent effects of these characteristics, which may have led to the mixed outcomes in this review. It will be important to systematically vary these feedback characteristics to determine optimal combinations, as some of these characteristics may have small but potentially additive effects.

This review only included studies that specifically compared different feedback conditions and not intervention packages, to isolate effects of feedback provision and different forms of feedback. However, different BCTs included in an intervention may interact since they link to or build on one another. For example, feedback provision may boost the effectiveness of other BCTs such as goal-setting since it may allow participants to identify changes that are most urgently needed or easiest to achieve [60]. Potential interactions between BCTs may also explain why Fanning [51] (which also used goal-setting) reported relatively large effects of feedback on changes in physical activity, while other studies (which did not use goal-setting) produced smaller effects.

Based on evaluations of feedback provision reported in a small number of included studies, it can be concluded that feedback provision is a desired and well-received study component, which mirrors previous research [59]. Surprisingly, in some of these studies, feedback provision did not improve intervention effectiveness despite the study participants reporting to find it useful, perhaps because feedback sometimes focuses on what the participant is doing well and maintains a human connection in some studies. On the other hand, previous research has pointed out that feedback may not always be beneficial; depending on the valence, it may also be seen as demotivating and so promote disengagement – rather than engagement – with the intervention [61]. In addition, the studies that examined the effect of feedback on retention did not find benefits [29, 41, 42]. These findings underline that feedback needs to be carefully crafted to achieve its desired effects of promoting intervention engagement and effectiveness.

Despite the systematic approach to this review, there are limitations that are important to note. First, the details on feedback provided in studies was often unavailable, which complicates the interpretation of the findings. Second, some of the interventions were extremely short (i.e., 2 days [25]) and most interventions were less than 12 weeks, so may not have been long enough to adequately test the feedback effect. In addition, some of the outcomes we examined were too different to include in additional meta-analyses. Furthermore, many studies had to be excluded because they tested intervention packages, which makes it difficult to estimate effects of individual intervention components. Third, the vast majority of included studies did not conduct sensitivity analyses to test for potential demographic differences in effects, and many included samples that were predominantly female, well educated, and white. This review thus cannot speak to the generalizability of the findings to deprived populations. Future research needs to address this issue, since engagement with and effectiveness of behavioral interventions likely are not equal for all [62, 63]. Finally, there was a high risk of bias in the majority of the studies, reflecting changing trends in pre-registration of analyses. In the future, rigorous experimental research using appropriate study designs such as factorial trials are needed to examine optimal feedback components further.

However, there are also strengths of this study. The design and conduct of the literature searches by an experienced medical librarian, the inclusion of 5 literature databases, and the use of forward and backward citation searches, which led to a comprehensive set of literature upon which to perform the review. Additionally, consistent with open science principles, we have reported the raw data on the OSF website. Finally, two reviewers independently coded all of the studies.

Conclusion

This review underlines the importance of feedback as a behavior change technique in interventions, but also clearly indicates that greater detail should be provided in scientific manuscripts regarding the feedback components (including examples and potentially screenshots) and frequency. In addition, more research is needed on how feedback is best generated (i.e., what can be generated by an algorithm and what potentially cannot) and presented to maximize intervention effectiveness.

Availability of data and materials

Raw data and analysis scripts are provided on the Open Science Framework (OSF; https://osf.io/j9duf/).

Change history

References

  1. World Health Organization. Obesity and Overweight. 2021. Accessed May 3, 2022.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Jensen MD, et al. 2013 AHA/ACC/TOS guideline for the management of overweight and obesity in adults: a report of the American College of Cardiology/American Heart Association Task Force on Practice Guidelines and The Obesity Society. J Am Coll Cardiol. 2014;63(25 Part B):2985–3023.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Wadden TA, Foster GD. Behavioral treatment of obesity. Med Clin North Am. 2000;84(2):441–61.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Goldstein SP, et al. Associations between self-monitoring and weight change in behavioral weight loss interventions. Health Psychol. 2019;38(12):1128.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Burke LE, Wang J, Sevick MA. Self-monitoring in weight loss: a systematic review of the literature. J Am Diet Assoc. 2011;111(1):92–102.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Michie S, et al. Effective techniques in healthy eating and physical activity interventions: a meta-regression. Health Psychol. 2009;28(6):690.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Patel ML, Wakayama LN, Bennett GG. Self-monitoring via digital health in weight loss interventions: a systematic review among adults with overweight or obesity. Obesity. 2021;29(3):478–99.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Harvey J, et al. Log often, lose more: Electronic dietary self-monitoring for weight loss. Obesity. 2019;27(3):380–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Krukowski RA, et al. Patterns of success: online self-monitoring in a web-based behavioral weight control program. Health Psychol. 2013;32(2):164.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Brockmann AN, Eastman A, Ross KM. Frequency and consistency of self-weighing to promote weight-loss maintenance. Obesity. 2020;28(7):1215–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Raber M, et al. A systematic review of the use of dietary self-monitoring in behavioural weight loss interventions: delivery, intensity and effectiveness. Public Health Nutr. 2021;24(17):5885–913.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Bandura A. Health promotion from the perspective of social cognitive theory. Psychol Health. 1998;13(4):623–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Bandura A. Social cognitive theory of self-regulation. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process. 1991;50(2):248–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Chhabria K, et al. The assessment of supportive accountability in adults seeking obesity treatment: psychometric validation study. J Med Internet Res. 2020;22(7):e17967.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Mohr D, Cuijpers P, Lehman K. Supportive accountability: a model for providing human support to enhance adherence to eHealth interventions. J Med Internet Res. 2011;13(1):e30.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Consolvo S, et al. Designing for healthy lifestyles: Design considerations for mobile technologies to encourage consumer health and wellness. 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Michie S, et al. The behavior change technique taxonomy (v1) of 93 hierarchically clustered techniques: building an international consensus for the reporting of behavior change interventions. Ann Behav Med. 2013;46(1):81–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Berry R, Kassavou A, Sutton S. Does self-monitoring diet and physical activity behaviors using digital technology support adults with obesity or overweight to lose weight? A systematic literature review with meta-analysis. Obes Rev. 2021;22(10):e13306.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Sanders JP, et al. Devices for self-monitoring sedentary time or physical activity: a scoping review. J Med Internet Res. 2016;18(5): e5373.

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  20. Sherrington A, et al. Systematic review and meta-analysis of internet-delivered interventions providing personalized feedback for weight loss in overweight and obese adults. Obes rev. 2016;17(6):541–51.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Schembre SM, et al. Just-in-time feedback in diet and physical activity interventions: systematic review and practical design framework. J Med Internet Res. 2018;20(3):e8701.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Fahey MC, et al. Counselor efficiency at providing feedback in a technology-based behavioral weight loss intervention: Longitudinal analysis. JMIR Formative Research. 2021;5(5):e23974.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Krukowski R, et al. Importance of multiple reinforcing comments and areas for change in optimizing dietary and exercise self-monitoring feedback in behavioral weight loss programs: Factorial design. J Med Internet Res. 2020;22(11):e18104.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Jauho A-M, et al. Effect of wrist-worn activity monitor feedback on physical activity behavior: a randomized controlled trial in Finnish young men. Preventive medicine reports. 2015;2:628–34.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Lukkahatai N, et al. Feasibility of using mobile technology to improve physical activity among people living with diabetes in Asia. Asian/Pacific Island Nurs J. 2021;5(4):236.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Martin A, et al. Feasibility of a real-time self-monitoring device for sitting less and moving more: a randomised controlled trial. BMJ Open Sport Exerc Med. 2017;3(1):e000285.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Burke LE, et al. The effect of electronic self-monitoring on weight loss and dietary intake: a randomized behavioral weight loss trial. Obesity. 2011;19(2):338–44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Burke LE, Sereika SM, Parmanto B, Bizhanova Z, Kariuki JK, Cheng J, Beatrice B, Loar I, Pulantara IW, Wang Y, Cedillo M. Effect of tailored, daily feedback with lifestyle self-monitoring on weight loss: The SMARTER randomized clinical trial. Obesity. 2022;30(1):75–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. West DS, Krukowski RA, Stansbury ML, Ogden D, Borden J, Harvey JR. Examining weekly facilitated group sessions and counselor-crafted self-monitoring feedback on treatment outcome in digital weight control: A pilot factorial study. Obes Sci Pract. 2022;8(4):433–41.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Michie S, et al. Behaviour change techniques: the development and evaluation of a taxonomic method for reporting and describing behaviour change interventions (a suite of five studies involving consensus methods, randomised controlled trials and analysis of qualitative data). Health Technol Assess. 2015;19(99):1–188.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Borenstein M, et al. Effect sizes based on means. Introduction to meta-analysis. 2009. p. 21–32.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  32. Viechtbauer W. Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. J Stat Softw. 2010;36(3):1–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Duval S, Tweedie R. Trim and fill: a simple funnel-plot–based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics. 2000;56(2):455–63.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Higgins JP, et al. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ. 2003;327(7414):557–60.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Harrer, M., et al., Doing meta-analysis with R: A hands-on guide. New York: Chapman and Hall/CRC; 2021.

  36. Prestwich A, et al. Do web-based competitions promote physical activity? Randomized controlled trial. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2017;29:1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Sterne JA, et al. RoB 2: a revised tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ. 2019;366:l4898.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Egger M, et al. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ. 1997;315(7109):629–34.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Sterne, J.A. and M. Egger, Regression methods to detect publication and other bias in meta-analysis. Publication bias in meta-analysis: Prevention, assessment and adjustments. Chichester: Wiley; 2005: p. 99–110.

  40. Blanson Henkemans OA, et al. An online lifestyle diary with a persuasive computer assistant providing feedback on self-management. Technol Health Care. 2009;17(3):253–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Tate DF, Jackvony EH, Wing RR. A randomized trial comparing human e-mail counseling, computer-automated tailored counseling, and no counseling in an Internet weight loss program. Arch Intern Med. 2006;166(15):1620–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Burke LE, et al. The SMARTER pilot study: testing feasibility of real-time feedback for dietary self-monitoring. Prev Med Rep. 2017;6:278–85.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Beleigoli A, et al. Personalized web-based weight loss behavior change program with and without dietitian online coaching for adults with overweight and obesity: randomized controlled trial. J Med Internet Res. 2020;22(11):e17494.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Godino JG, et al. Impact of personalised feedback about physical activity on change in objectively measured physical activity (the FAB study): a randomised controlled trial. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(9):e75398.

    Article  ADS  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Lawrie S, et al. Evaluation of a smartwatch-based intervention providing feedback of daily activity within a research-naive stroke ward: a pilot randomised controlled trial. Pilot Feasibility Stud. 2018;4(1):1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Kerrigan SG, et al. Project Step: A randomized controlled trial investigating the effects of frequent feedback and contingent incentives on physical activity. J Phys Act Health. 2021;18(3):247–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Rabbi M, et al. Automated personalized feedback for physical activity and dietary behavior change with mobile phones: a randomized controlled trial on adults. JMIR Mhealth Uhealth. 2015;3(2):e4160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Prestwich A, et al. An experimental test of control theory-based interventions for physical activity. Br J Health Psychol. 2016;21(4):812–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Wang J, et al. Effect of adherence to self-monitoring of diet and physical activity on weight loss in a technology-supported behavioral intervention. Patient Prefer Adherence. 2012;6:221.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Conroy MB, et al. Physical activity self-monitoring and weight loss: 6-month results of the SMART trial. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2011;43(8):1568.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Fanning J, et al. A smartphone “app”-delivered randomized factorial trial targeting physical activity in adults. J Behav Med. 2017;40(5):712–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Paschali AA, et al. Accelerometer feedback to promote physical activity in adults with type 2 diabetes: a pilot study. Percept Mot Skills. 2005;100(1):61–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Kim H, Kang M. A tailored domain-specific intervention using contextual information about sedentary behavior to reduce sedentary time: A Bayesian approach. Meas Phys Educ Exerc Sci. 2021;25(2):171–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Ambeba EJ, et al. The use of mHealth to deliver tailored messages reduces reported energy and fat intake. J Cardiovasc Nurs. 2015;30(1):35.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Turk MW, et al. Self-monitoring as a mediator of weight loss in the SMART randomized clinical trial. Int J Behav Med. 2013;20(4):556–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Higgins, J.P. and S. Green, Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions (Version 5.1.0) [updated March 2011]. Available from www.handbook.cochrane.org.

  57. Foley P, et al. Track: a randomized controlled trial of a digital health obesity treatment intervention for medically vulnerable primary care patients. Contemp Clin Trials. 2016;48:12–20.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  58. van der Haar S, et al. Exploring the potential of personalized dietary advice for health improvement in motivated individuals with premetabolic syndrome: pretest-posttest study. JMIR Formative Research. 2021;5(6):e25043.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. König LM, et al. Barriers to and facilitators for using nutrition apps: systematic review and conceptual framework. JMIR Mhealth Uhealth. 2021;9(6):e20037.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Wang Y, König LM, Reiterer H. A smartphone app to support sedentary behavior change by visualizing personal mobility patterns and action planning (SedVis): Development and pilot study. JMIR Formative Research. 2021;5(1):e15369.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Attig C, Franke T. Abandonment of personal quantification: A review and empirical study investigating reasons for wearable activity tracking attrition. Comput Hum Behav. 2020;102:223–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Szinay D, et al. Is the uptake, engagement, and effectiveness of exclusively mobile interventions for the promotion of weight-related behaviors equal for all? A systematic review. Obes Rev. 2023;24(3):e13542.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Birch JM, et al. A systematic review of inequalities in the uptake of, adherence to, and effectiveness of behavioral weight management interventions in adults. Obes Rev. 2022;23(6):e13438.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

Dr. Krukowski is funded by the National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases (R01 DK104872, R01 DK107747, R01 DK135227) and the National Cancer Institute (R01CA218155). This work was supported by a short-term grant from the University of Bayreuth Humboldt Centre. The funding agencies were not involved in any aspect of this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

RAK and LK conceptualized and designed the study. The literature search was performed by AD. Screening, article review, and data extraction were performed by RAK and LK. Data analysis was performed by LK. RAK drafted the manuscript. All authors revised the manuscript and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rebecca A. Krukowski.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

Drs. Krukowski and König serve on the editorial board for the International Journal for Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity. The authors declare that they have no other competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

The original version of this article was revised: The authors updated the meta-analysis and thus updated the text and figures accordingly.

Supplementary Information

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Krukowski, R.A., Denton, A.H. & König, L.M. Impact of feedback generation and presentation on self-monitoring behaviors, dietary intake, physical activity, and weight: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 21, 3 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12966-023-01555-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12966-023-01555-6

Keywords