Open Access

Meeting in the middle: motivational interviewing and self-determination theory

International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity20129:25

https://doi.org/10.1186/1479-5868-9-25

Received: 22 December 2011

Accepted: 2 March 2012

Published: 2 March 2012

Motivational interviewing (MI) is indeed a "bottom-up" model that emerged from practical experience in the field of alcohol treatment. The original description of MI [1] suggested some links to social psychological theories, but focused on an intuitive approach in treating alcohol problems for which there was at the time no empirical support. Our subsequent volumes [2, 3] have similarly focused on clinical applications without proposing an underlying theory of treatment or change.

In part this reflects our own temperaments, preferring intuitive to rational-deductive ways of knowing [4, 5], with a focus on the "real" world of clinical practice. We are "bottom-up" people. Much of what we have done in our careers has sprung from efforts to deal with practical problems that clinicians encounter in their daily work [6]. The world of academia, in contrast, tends to place a high premium on starting from coherent theory and rationally deriving hypotheses that will be tested to either confirm or revise the theory. This has simply never been a forte or primary scientific interest for either of us, to the dismay of some of our mentors and colleagues. We have preferred instead to move between the context of discovery and the context of justification [7] - deriving intuitive hypotheses from clinical experience, submitting them to the verification of scientific method, and then going back to the drawing board to try again. Over time, this approach may lead to the development of a higher-order theory as a byproduct [8]. The rigor of scientific method is equally important in both approaches. They differ in the source of hypotheses: intuitive experience versus rational deduction from a pre-existing theory. Both approaches have value and a long tradition in the history of science. Whether either one is in some sense superior to the other is a value judgment that we do not wish to make.

The history of MI, however, does suggest potential value in beginning from clinical intuition. A large evidence base comprising more than 200 randomized clinical trials has emerged, showing positive effects (albeit inconsistent) across many health problem areas. Well before this evidence base accumulated, however, MI disseminated readily and rapidly by word of mouth among clinicians, who are drawn to it not just from the clinical trials but because, for the lack of a better term, they seem to "recognize" it. It feels intuitively sound based on their own experience. This kind of practice-based evidence is also important, and needs to be compared, tested and refined with clinical trials. Hall [9] suggested a similar two-way street in psychotherapy research with cultural minorities. Evidence-based treatments are worth trying in populations where they have not yet been tested [10], and there is also a need for scientific study of the intuitive interventions that have arisen from an indigenous culture's own wisdom and experience.

So what about Self-Determination Theory (SDT) that grew up independently from MI, but bears a certain family resemblance? There may be a natural fit [11]. MI has lacked a well-developed theory to rationalize its efficacy. SDT has focused less on refining specific clinical procedures for putting it into practice. A marriage may be premature, but the flirtation is not. The three human hungers emphasized in SDT - autonomy, relatedness, and competence - are all directly addressed in MI. More than most psychotherapies, MI assumes, respects, and implicitly relies on volition to instigate self-regulation [12]. The emerging psycholinguistic "mechanisms" of MI [8, 13, 14] can be linked to the more general development of volition and self-regulation through language [15]. The relational component of MI also appears to be important [8], consistent with SDT. Supporting autonomy is a key element in the underlying spirit of MI. SDT and MI, it would seem, have much to learn from each other.

SDT also holds promise for improving our understanding of MI. A puzzling aspect in MI clinical research is the inconsistency of its outcomes. There are many positive trials, but also an impressive number of negative trials, including some of our own [16]. SDT may help to clarify the conditions that contribute to the effectiveness of MI in practice. MI has been faulted for underemphasizing social context [17], a factor that is clearly integrated in SDT, and a well-supported theory never hurts the academic credibility of any psychotherapy.

Is SDT more than just another pair of theoretical glasses through which to view the phenomena of MI? Will SDT lead to unique testable hypotheses that teach us important things about MI that we didn't already know? It remains to be seen. We do not propose to develop such a systematic integration ourselves, but we gladly offer our support to those whose aptitudes and inclinations lie in this direction.

Authors’ Affiliations

(1)
The University of New Mexico
(2)
School of Medicine, Cardiff University

References

  1. Miller WR: Motivational interviewing with problem drinkers. Behavioural Psychotherapy. 1983, 11: 141-172.View ArticleGoogle Scholar
  2. Miller RW, Rollnick S: Motivational Interviewing: Preparing people to change addictive behavior. 1991, New York: Guilford PressGoogle Scholar
  3. Miller RW, Rollnick S: Motivational Interviewing: Preparing People for Change. 2002, New York: Guilford PressGoogle Scholar
  4. Polanyi M: The tacit dimension. 1966, Chicago: University of Chicago PressGoogle Scholar
  5. Quenk NL: Essentials of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator assessment. 2009, New York: John Wiley & SonsGoogle Scholar
  6. Miller WR, Meyers RJ, Tonigan JS: Engaging the unmotivated in treatment for alcohol problems: a comparison of three strategies for intervention through family members. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1999, 67: 688-697.View ArticleGoogle Scholar
  7. Reichenbach H: Experience and prediction. 1938, Chicago: University of Chicago PressGoogle Scholar
  8. Miller WR, Rose GS: Toward a theory of motivational interviewing. Am Psychol. 2009, 64: 527-537.View ArticleGoogle Scholar
  9. Hall GC: Psychotherapy research with ethnic minorities: empirical, ethical, and conceptual issues. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2001, 69: 502-510.View ArticleGoogle Scholar
  10. Miller WR, Villanueva M, Tonigan JS, Cuzmar I: Are special treatments needed for special populations?. Alcoholism Treatment Quarterly. 2007, 25: 63-78. 10.1300/J020v25n04_05.View ArticleGoogle Scholar
  11. Markland D, Ryan RM, Tobin VJ, Rollnick S: Motivational Interviewing and Self-Determination Theory. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 2005, 24: 811-831. 10.1521/jscp.2005.24.6.811.View ArticleGoogle Scholar
  12. Miller WR, Atencio DJ: Free will as a proportion of variance. Are we free? Psychology and free will. Edited by: Baer J, Kaufman JC, Baumeister RF. 2008, New York: Oxford University Press, 275-295.View ArticleGoogle Scholar
  13. Amrhein PC, Miller WR, Yahne CE, Palmer M, Fulcher L: Client commitment language during motivational interviewing predicts drug use outcomes. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2003, 71: 862-878.View ArticleGoogle Scholar
  14. Moyers TB, Martin T, Christopher PJ, Houck JM, Tonigan JS, Amrhein PC: Client language as a mediator of motivational interviewing efficacy: where is the evidence?. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2007, 31: 40s-47s. 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00492.x.View ArticleGoogle Scholar
  15. Diaz RM, Winsler A, Atencio DJ, HArbers K: Mediation of self-regulation through the use of private speech. International Journal of Cognitive Education and Mediated Learning. 1992, 2: 155-167.Google Scholar
  16. Miller WR, Yahne CE, Tonigan JS: Motivational interviewing in drug abuse services: a randomized trial. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2003, 71: 754-763.View ArticleGoogle Scholar
  17. Miller WR, Rose GS: Motivational interviewing in relational context. American Psychologist. 2010, 65: 298-299.View ArticleGoogle Scholar

Copyright

© Miller and Rollnick; licensee BioMed Central Ltd. 2012

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Advertisement