Skip to main content

Methodological considerations for observational coding of eating and feeding behaviors in children and their families

Abstract

Background

Behavioral coding of videotaped eating and feeding interactions can provide researchers with rich observational data and unique insights into eating behaviors, food intake, food selection as well as interpersonal and mealtime dynamics of children and their families. Unlike self-report measures of eating and feeding practices, the coding of videotaped eating and feeding behaviors can allow for the quantitative and qualitative examinations of behaviors and practices that participants may not self-report. While this methodology is increasingly more common, behavioral coding protocols and methodology are not widely shared in the literature. This has important implications for validity and reliability of coding schemes across settings. Additional guidance on how to design, implement, code and analyze videotaped eating and feeding behaviors could contribute to advancing the science of behavioral nutrition. The objectives of this narrative review are to review methodology for the design, operationalization, and coding of videotaped behavioral eating and feeding data in children and their families, and to highlight best practices.

Methods

When capturing eating and feeding behaviors through analysis of videotapes, it is important for the study and coding to be hypothesis driven. Study design considerations include how to best capture the target behaviors through selection of a controlled experimental laboratory environment versus home mealtime, duration of video recording, number of observations to achieve reliability across eating episodes, as well as technical issues in video recording and sound quality. Study design must also take into account plans for coding the target behaviors, which may include behavior frequency, duration, categorization or qualitative descriptors. Coding scheme creation and refinement occur through an iterative process. Reliability between coders can be challenging to achieve but is paramount to the scientific rigor of the methodology. Analysis approach is dependent on the how data were coded and collapsed.

Conclusions

Behavioral coding of videotaped eating and feeding behaviors can capture rich data “in-vivo” that is otherwise unobtainable from self-report measures. While data collection and coding are time-intensive the data yielded can be extremely valuable. Additional sharing of methodology and coding schemes around eating and feeding behaviors could advance the science and field.

Background

Observational measurement of eating and feeding provides a window into behaviors of children and their families that participants may not self-report. This methodology provides the researcher with a unique glimpse into eating behaviors, which can yield rich quantitative and qualitative data. A wide variety of behaviors can be observationally captured. For example, one can examine individual eating behaviors or familial interactions around food, how much food is consumed under certain conditions, environmental factors (where a meal is eaten, how it is served, who is present at the meal), and linguistics of conversation around food. Most studies of eating, feeding and mealtime behaviors have used self-report measures [1, 2], which although less resource intensive, is limited by social desirability bias [3,4,5]. Observational methodology can capture behaviors of which people are unaware or do not to report, or that are not explicitly asked about. Some prior work [3, 5, 6] has found that self-report and observational measures of eating and feeding behaviors are only weakly correlated. These weak associations may be because these different methodologies are measuring different constructs, with questionnaires measuring parents’ perceptions of behaviors, and videotaped observations measuring objective observations. The weak correlations could also be due to social desirability bias on questionnaires, or the Hawthorne effect [7] that comes with knowing one is being video recorded. Other work [8] has found good correlations between parent report and observed mealtime behaviors, when the constructs from both measures are tightly mapped and highly concordant. It is important to note that self-report measures only capture constructs and behaviors that the questionnaire specifically asks about, and therefore can leave aspects of eating or feeding interactions unquantifiable. Video recorded observational techniques allow for the preservation of a nuanced interaction, which can be reviewed when new questions arise, and re-examined for different facets of behavior. The strengths and limitations of employing observational and self-report methodology for eating and feeding behaviors are summarized in Table 1. The researcher must carefully weigh the strengths and limitations of each methodology, while considering how best to test their hypothesis when choosing their methodological approach. In summary, recognizing that all methods have strengths and limitations, observational methodology captures unique aspects of eating and feeding that enrich our understanding of these behaviors and the contexts in which they occur.

Table 1 Advantages and challenges of self-report measures vs. observational coding for measurement of eating and feeding behaviors

Despite the many benefits of observational coding of eating and feeding behaviors, there are potential challenges which deserve consideration in study design and planning. Herein, we will briefly describe some of the challenges. Technical considerations of video recording eating behaviors should not be underestimated. These include ensuring adequate video and audio quality to capture desired behaviors, getting video recording devices to participants along with instructions for set-up, recording and return of devices and data, in addition to storage and management of video files. This can be logistically challenging, and requires a detail oriented staff who can communicate well with participants throughout the process to maintain data quality. Obtaining video recordings of eating and feeding behaviors is more time and resource intensive than questionnaires as it requires more participant time, more involved data management and logistics, and coding of behaviors. Furthermore, as described below, coding behaviors can be challenging and time intensive. As previously mentioned, the Hawthorne effect with regard to participants’ behavior while being video recorded will always be a limitation. However, most of these challenges are surmountable with careful design and flexible thinking and, in our opinion, are outweighed by the quality of the data the can be yielded from observational methodology.

The objectives of this paper are to outline methodological considerations when designing, implementing, coding and analyzing data from a study using observational coding of eating and feeding behaviors in children and their families, and highlight best practices for doing so. An overview of the steps in such a study is presented in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

A flowchart of basic steps in the study of observational eating and feeding behaviors

Designing the behavioral protocol to be observed

When designing a behavioral protocol to capture eating or feeding behaviors, the authors believe that it is important to start with a hypothesis. What is the behavior or variable that the researcher is most interested in? The answer to this question will guide elements of the behavioral protocol such as whether the video recording should be in a naturalistic environment (i.e., at home) or in a laboratory setting. If, for instance, the researcher is interested in testing if maternal-child food talk varies based on portion size of a meal presented, then researcher may opt to conduct their study in a laboratory based setting in which food can be precisely portioned, and audiovisual equipment can capture the verbal and physical interaction. If, on the other hand, the researcher is interested in examining whether the structure of family meals (e.g., location of the meal, whether the family says a blessing before the meal, if the meal is eaten at a table, etc.) is associated with child weight status, obtaining videos from a naturalistic environment would be more appropriate. Being hypothesis driven will also be important in determining the parameters placed around the video recording – including how the food is presented, types and portions of foods served, length of video recording or food exposure, people present during food consumption, etc. There are many considerations that contribute to careful design of a protocol, which are essential to ensuring that the data obtained will be optimal quality to test the hypothesis.

The setting

The setting of a protocol to capture eating and feeding behaviors is a fundamental consideration in study design, as the setting may have a strong influence on participants’ behavior [9]. The setting of a protocol can be a naturalistic environment (e.g., in the home or at a restaurant), a laboratory, or a semi-naturalistic environment with controlled elements (e.g., a simulated laboratory restaurant). The advantages and disadvantages to each type of setting, (summarized in Table 2) should be carefully weighed when choosing a study design to best test a hypothesis. Naturalistic settings theoretically capture people exhibiting more naturalistic behaviors [10]. For instance, in a home mealtime recording participants may be more at ease and may be more likely to eat and interact in a more natural manner, allowing for the capture of data that is closer to “real life”. Additional advantages include being able to capture the eating interaction between multiple family members or participants more easily than arranging for a laboratory visit for multiple people. The researcher may also gain insights into the ways in which food is prepared and served, who is present at the meal, and what additional events are going on at the same time (e.g., TV on, people coming in and out of the home, mobile devices present, etc.) [11]. The day-to-day variability of eating in naturalistic settings is another important consideration [12, 13] – (e.g., just because a family orders in pizza one night, or has grandmother over to eat with the family, does not necessarily mean that this is an everyday practice). Additional disadvantages to observations in a naturalistic setting include the lack of experimental control with regard to the ways in which the foods are served or eaten, difficulties in measurement of food intake, and who is present at the mealtime [14]. Technical considerations around video recording are also a major consideration, as sound quality, lighting, movement of the participants during the mealtime out of the view of the camera, and background noise may all interfere with the quality of data captured. Some of these issues can be overcome if a research assistant delivers the video recorder to the participant(s) and instructs the family on its use, retrieving it after the first session or after subsequent sessions [8], however this may come at the expense of social desirability bias, as it is an additional reminder to the participants that they are being observed, and is labor intensive [15]. In addition, it is important to consider that for all individuals present in a video recording for research purposes, informed consent or assent must be obtained [16].

Table 2 Advantages and challenges of different settings for the observation of behavioral eating and feeding

In a laboratory setting, the researcher has much more control over the experimental design and implementation [17]. The manner in which the protocol is set up can be standardized, including the physical environment, the ways in which food is presented (e.g., buffet style, family style, individualized portions), who is present for the protocol, and length of time the participants are exposed to a food. In addition, the type and amount of food served can be carefully controlled and standardized across participants. The researcher can also manipulate the independent variables, and can more accurately measure intake [17]. Technical issues such as video and sound quality are easily controlled, and the researcher can troubleshoot any issues that arise in the moment. However, as in all data collection relying on video recorded data, the laboratory setting is not exempt from technical challenges (e.g., battery or power issues, video recorded malfunctioning, etc.). A challenge of the laboratory environment is that participants may interact with each other and food differently in an unfamiliar environment [18]. A final setting, the semi-naturalistic environment, consists of a replication of a naturalistic environment in a laboratory setting that allows for tighter control of experimental and technical elements, but may put the participants at ease. Examples of these types of arrangements include a laboratory designed like a kitchen and dining room [19] or a simulated fast-food restaurant [20], equipped with “hidden” cameras. While the participants know about the presence of the cameras, they may eat and interact in a more naturalistic manner due to the camera not being in front of them on a tripod as a constant reminder.

For all settings, the impact of social desirability bias and the Hawthorne effect [7] on participants’ behavior due to the presence of a video camera is difficult to gauge–participants may behave differently because they are aware of being observed, to avoid embarrassment, or to “look good” to the researcher, which may differ from their typical behavior. Observations in naturalistic or semi-naturalistic environments may theoretically lessen this effect, but cannot eliminate it.

When choosing a setting, it is important for the researcher to weigh the strengths and weaknesses of each setting type, as mentioned above. The authors feel that a guiding question can be whether or not the hypothesis requires testing of a tightly controlled exposure (e.g., time, food type, amount consumed, presentation, etc.). If the hypothesis requires a tightly controlled exposure, then a laboratory setting is likely best. However, if the research design does not require a tightly controlled exposure and the researcher wishes to capture more “typical” behavior, then a naturalistic setting should be chosen. Lastly, the researcher may choose a semi-naturalistic setting if they want to both control for an exposure as well as capture more typical behavior.

The set-up

Regardless of the setting of the video recording, the authors suggest that it is important to explain the purpose of the protocol to participants in a scripted and standardized manner, carefully veiling the purpose of the study and the hypothesis being tested. The participants should not be aware of the specific goals of the study or the specific behaviors being observed, to avoid biasing their behavior. Special attention needs to be paid so that the script is not leading, but also meets the ethical obligation to inform the participants about the study. For instance, if the amount of food consumed in a particular condition is the outcome to be measured, it is important to avoid implying to the participants that they are expected to eat the food (which could serve to increase their consumption). Rather, instructing the participants to “eat as much or as little as you’d like”, allows for more flexibility in the participants’ behavior. In a home mealtime recording, the same considerations apply when explaining the purpose of the video recording to the participants. The authors have found that explanations can be as vague as, “we want to learn how families do dinner”.

Technical issues

When video recording, the frame of the video should to be standardized so that the desired behavior is captured [21]. The authors have found that there are many considerations for the researcher that should be outlined explicitly in the protocol to optimize quality data collection. The researcher must consider if they want just an individual in the frame, an entire group or family or a dyad. If multiple people are present, do they all need to be facing the camera, or is it acceptable for some people to have their backs to the camera? If there is an index participant who is the focus of the study, can other people be eating with him or her, and does the index participant need to be in the center of the frame? Is it important to have the food being eaten in view of the camera, and if so, does this include the plate/dish/wrapper from which the individuals are eating, and/or any larger serving containers? Does the view of the participants need to be unobstructed?

The length of the recording should be specified [22]. Does the researcher need a fixed length of time for the eating segment (this is easy to do in a laboratory setting), or should the participants be allowed to decide when they are “done”? In a naturalistic setting, clear instructions should be provided to the participants with regard to how to set up the video recorder, when to turn it on or off and how to set up the video frame. In a laboratory setting, these details should be articulated in a standardized written protocol. Files of the video recordings, regardless of setting, should be labeled with the participant’s unique identifying number after the file is obtained.

Lighting and sound quality can vary greatly in naturalistic settings [21] and, in the author’s experience, are more easily controlled in a laboratory setting. There are additional challenges of returning the video camera, or digital files containing the video recording to the research team when recordings are completed by participants in naturalistic settings. Associated costs and logistics must be considered in study planning.

Special considerations for naturalistic settings

There are some additional special considerations for observed eating or feeding behaviors in naturalistic settings. Given variability across days, it is important to consider how many events or meals are needed to achieve acceptable reliability across meals. In addition, the number of meals necessary to achieve reliability across meals depends on the behavior being evaluated. It is also important to obtain a sense from the participants as to whether or not the meal or eating interaction was “typical” for the family. This is important to know given the inherent variability of naturalistic settings (e.g., child is unwell or has a tantrum, unexpected mealtime visitor, etc.) and the common desire for studying “typical” behavior.

After the meal, it is often necessary to document who was present and what was served, as this may not be apparent when reviewing the video recordings. The authors have found that this can be done via phone call by a research assistant, or self-recorded by the participant. This information should be collected in a standardized manner across mealtimes or eating events and participants.

Lastly, the authors have encountered instances in which interactions or behaviors captured on video cause concern for the safety of participants, including concerns for child abuse or neglect. Prior to implementing a protocol and collecting data, it is important to have a plan in place regarding how to report these concerns to the appropriate agency to protect the well-being of participants. In the United States, researchers are Mandated Reporters [23], meaning that the law requires researchers to report their suspicions of child abuse or neglect to Children’s Protective Services. The laws and agencies may vary in different countries.

Coding

Coding is a rigorous, systematic and often iterative process that consists of identifying a target behavior or event, and the best way to capture it from video recordings [10]. It is important to consider the potential coding approach during study design [10], and then revisit the feasibility of this approach through review of videos prior to development of the coding scheme. If an event or behavior does not occur in the video recordings as frequently as the researcher had hypothesized (or occurs in nearly every video), the researcher would be prudent to reconsider the approach prior to starting to code.

Questions that need to be answered early in this process to inform the development of a coding strategy include: which individuals (e.g., an individual, a family, a dyadic interaction?) and/or what events (e.g., food being served, mobile device use while eating, conversation, bites of food) are being coded [24]? If events or behaviors are contingent upon one another (e.g., child compliance with maternal prompts to take a bite), multiple behaviors will need to be coded in relation to one another in time.

Coding approaches: Time and frequency considerations

The time interval for coding must be carefully selected. Videos can be coded as a whole, in intervals or as event based. The selection of coding interval depends on the level of detail that the researcher wishes to capture. For instance, to capture general information about a family mealtime, a researcher may choose to code a video in its entirety for simple binary or categorical variables (e.g., in what room of the house did the meal take place? Which family members were present at the meal? Did the mother sit down at the table? Was the TV audible)? More descriptive questions can be answered as well, (e.g., on a scale of 1–5, how oppositional was the child during the meal?) However, if a researcher desires to know more granular information about the meal or eating event, they may wish to use frequency-based, duration or interval coding.

Frequency or event-based coding records the occurrence of each event in a specific period. This coding approach is best used for discrete behaviors that are easily counted (i.e., each time a participant picks up his/her fork and puts food in his/her mouth), as well as shorter observational periods, as frequencies can be labor intensive to capture. If the hypothesis seeks to examine the temporal relationship of one event or behavior to another, it is important to also capture the timing of each event.

Duration coding captures the absolute value or percent of the time that a behavior occurs during an observation period (e.g., minutes in which the child is seated at the table during the meal). Duration coding can be used to capture the percentage of time that an individual is engaged in a specific behavior (e.g., percentage of the protocol that a mother was engaged with her mobile device). This type of coding is best used for behaviors that last more than a few seconds and that occur repeatedly throughout a meal or protocol.

Lastly, interval coding is an alternative approach that can alleviate some of the challenges of frequency or duration coding [10]. In this method, the researcher examines whether a behavior or event occurs at all in multiple smaller time intervals during the observation. For instance, a researcher may code whether or not a participant took a bite of food within each 10 s window of a 4-min video observation. This can provide an estimate for frequency and also duration.

Coding schemes and guides

Coding schemes can be created anew to answer specific questions or previously developed coding schemes may be applied [24]. While many prior studies have coded behavioral aspects of eating and feeding behaviors, few have published their coding manuals in detail. This may be due to word count limitations in print journals (a factor that is less pressing in the age of primarily digital publication), as well as authors’ desires to protect their intellectual property. If a coding scheme is not published, authors will often provide it upon request. The authors argue that in order for the science eating and feeding behavior observations to advance swiftly, more authors should share their coding methodology using open access platforms. Existing coding schemes that capture mealtime and/or eating behaviors are presented in Table 3. These coding schemes may need to be modified depending on the hypotheses being tested [24]. Chorney et al. [24], have published a detailed guide to developing and refining coding schemes, which is a helpful resource.

Table 3 Eating and feeding behavior coding schemes for children over 12 months of age, and their families

The authors have found that there is great value in developing a new coding scheme which can capture novel behaviors or events. To begin, the researcher should watch many video observations to get a sense of how frequent the behavior/event is and whether it is best captured qualitatively, categorically, or as a count. These observations will guide the time segment considerations in coding previously described. While watching the videos, the researcher should take notes to describe the behavior in a specific and detailed manner, defining the specific parameters of the behavior or event, and differentiating between categories if applicable. It is equally important to describe what does not qualify as meeting the specified parameters of a behavior (e.g., a sip of water does not count as a bite of food). This level of specificity will improve the likelihood that the codes will be applied in a reliable manner. Coding categories can be binary (e.g., did an event happen? Yes, vs No) or categorical (e.g., the child took a small, medium or large serving of food). An excessive number of categories will make reliability difficult to achieve, but an insufficient number of categories may lose important nuance in the data. After drafting a coding scheme, the researcher should apply it to several videos and refine it. Explicit directions should be included for the application of the coding scheme including where to access and store data, how many passes should be used to code, if all behaviors or events should be coded in the same pass or one at a time, etc.

Refining a coding scheme and reliability

Once the initial draft of a coding scheme is created, two to three coders should independently apply it to a preselected random sample of video observations to achieve familiarity with the coding scheme. The authors usually start with 5 observations. Coders then meet to compare their codes and discuss disagreements or points of confusion, which may lead to the modification of the coding scheme. This iterative process will continue, with the application and subsequent modification of the coding scheme, until the researcher feels that the final coding scheme has been developed. On all codes where disagreements occurred, the team must come to a consensus. Alternatively, one researcher can code all videos with a coding scheme, and a second coder can be brought in later to establish inter-rater reliability. This practice is sometimes necessary given limited resources or the complexity of a coding scheme, however can be higher risk if reliability cannot be established, and the original coding scheme needs to be modified which would result in re-coding of videos.

It is important to note that reliability can be difficult to establish at times, even for seemingly simply codes. Each coder brings with them their own set of experience and biases, which influence how they interpret and code behaviors. Even in simple coding approaches, such as frequency or event based coding, there can often be challenges in establishing reliability between raters, especially for rare events or if events are easily overlooked. Difficulty in establishing reliability may be due to a variety of factors such as a lack of explicit detail in the coding guide or need for more simplistic coding conceptualization, or a need for training sessions and discussion of disagreements in the coders. A resource recommended by the authors for improving and troubleshooting reliability issues for videotaped behavioral observations is Haidet et al., 2009 [22].

Once it seems that the coders are similarly interpreting the coding scheme, all coders should independently apply the coding scheme to 20–30% of video segments, again randomly sampled. Inter-rater reliability should be calculated based on a Cohen’s kappa [22] (for categorical codes) or an intraclass correlation coefficient (for continuous codes, such a Likert scales or counts), which should exceed 0.7 or 0.8, respectively, for each code [25]. If reliability is achieved, then the remainder of the videos may be coded by a single coder. If raters are not reliable for all codes, disagreements should be discussed, the coding scheme refined, and the process of independently applying the new coding scheme to new videos must be repeated. After reliability is established for all codes, the videos used for “training” and reliability establishment must be coded with the final coding scheme. Furthermore, it is important to periodically check inter-rater reliability to ensure no significant drift has occurred during coding of the corpus.

When selecting coders, it is best to train individuals who are blind to the research hypotheses, to avoid unintentional bias. It is important to consider the complexity and nuance of the behavior being coded, and whether a specific coder skill set or background is needed [24] (e.g. choosing between undergraduate students vs. doctoral students). It is also recommended to train more coders than the researcher anticipates needing to guard against staffing changes impeding progress.

Finally, the authors have found that it is useful to pause after coding about 25–30% of the corpus of videos and review the frequencies of behaviors being coded. Is this a rare event that is worth capturing at all? If so, should the definition be changed? Should the code be abandoned or the time segment broadened (e.g. does this behavior occur in this 5 s segment versus does this behavior ever happen at all in this video)? There is often a need to balance the labor and time that goes into coding an uncommon event with the value of that event data to the greater project.

Analyses

Once the data have been painstakingly coded, the next step is to analyze the data, which largely depends on how the data were coded [26]. While original hypotheses will drive the study design and therefore the analysis plan, there is still some flexibility. It is helpful to first examine the frequencies, means, ranges, and standard deviations of codes. For counts of behaviors, one can examine a summary score of all counts in a video segment (e.g., number of bites during a meal) latency to first event (e.g. time to first bite), or change in event rate over time (e.g., bite rate at the beginning of the meal vs middle vs the end). If interval coding was performed (i.e., whether a behavior was present (vs. not) in each 5 s interval), the proportion of time engaged in that behavior can be calculated. If certain behaviors or events occur infrequently, then code categories can be created (e.g., did the child ask for second helpings >5 times, 4–5 times, 1–3 times, or not at all). Categories can be collapsed (e.g., did the child ask for second helpings >3 times, 1–3 times, or never) or changed into whole video coding (e.g., did the child ever as for second helpings? Yes vs. no). Macro-level behavioral coding can be applied using a simple spreadsheet to capture codes, and video playing software. Micro-level coding such as time-window sequential analysis [27] will require additional training and use of coding software, such as Observer XT [28]. A detailed resource on analyzing data from behavioral observation is the text by Suen and Ary, 2014 [26].

Best practice highlights

The authors outline below what they consider to be best practices for observational coding of eating and feeding behaviors in children and their families.

  • Start with a hypothesis. The researcher should be sure to understand the question they are asking and how they want to test their hypothesis. This will guide selection of the setting.

  • Select the setting based on the hypothesis. If the researcher wants to examine a behavioral response to a specific exposure, then a tightly controlled laboratory experiment may be the best fit. Whereas if the observer wants to examine behaviors in a naturalistic setting, for instance differences in foods served when families eat at the kitchen table vs. other locations, then a naturalistic setting would be a better fi.

  • Carefully plan the protocol using an extensively detailed guide. The authors recommend writing out explicit step by step instructions that are detailed so that each protocol can be set up as similarity as possibly to improve scientific rigor. It is important to specify the “who, what, where, when” and especially the “how” of a behavioral protocol.

  • Pilot the protocol. As unforeseen events can occur even with the most seemingly straightforward protocols, it is important to pilot and refine the protocol before involving participants. This will highlight areas of challenge that can be avoided or minimized when collecting valuable participant data.

  • Double check video and audio recording devices. Prior to an encounter with a participant, it is paramount to ensure the recording devices are working properly to minimize technical issues that may impair data collection.

  • Review data once collection has started, pilot a coding scheme and adjust protocol as necessary. Once data collection has started, it is important to review the video recordings as well as develop and pilot a coding scheme. This will help the researcher understand if their protocol is successfully testing their hypothesis, or if changes need to be made before data collection is complete.

  • Train raters on reliably coding scheme application through an iterative process. This will likely involve training of the coder as well as multiple revisions of the coding scheme to add more detail, specificity around the concepts described

  • Trouble-shoot reliability issues. Reliability should be calculated at the beginning of coding as well as periodically throughout the coding process to protect against drift. When difficulties in reliability present it is important to examine the coding scheme (e.g. definitions of codes may need to be specified or categories may need to be collapsed), the coding approach (e.g. frequency based vs. binary coding, or widening a time interval if too narrow). It is important to examine both the coding scheme, as well as its application by the coder. Some coders may not have the necessary insights, observation skills or attention to detail to apply all coding schemes, therefore it behooves the researcher to train more coders than they think are necessary.

  • Analyze the data to test the hypothesis. At the end of the long road of observational data collection and coding, the analysis allows for the opportunity to test the hypothesis. Examine univariate data (e.g. frequencies, means, standard deviations) first. Categories with low frequencies may need to be collapsed into larger categories.

Conclusions

Observational coding of eating and feeding behaviors is a resource and time intensive process, but can yield invaluable data. Researchers can minimize the challenges, and maximize the benefits of this methodology by carefully considering their hypotheses prior to data collection, and designing their protocol and coding approach to address their questions. Additional sharing of methodology and coding schemes around eating and feeding behaviors could advance the science and field.

References

  1. Orrell-Valente JK, Hill LG, Brechwald WA, Dodge KA, Pettit GS, Bates JE. “Just three more bites”: an observational analysis of parents’ socialization of children's eating at mealtime. Appetite. 2007;48:37–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Faith MS, Scanlon KS, Birch LL, Francis LA, Sherry B. Parent-child feeding strategies and their relationships to child eating and weight status. Obes Res. 2004;12:1711–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Hebert JR, Clemow L, Pbert L, Ockene IS, Ockene JK. Social desirability bias in dietary self-report may compromise the validity of dietary intake measures. Int J Epidemiol. 1995;24:389–98.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Hebert JR, Ma Y, Clemow L, Ockene IS, Saperia G, Stanek IIIEJ, Merriam PA, Ockene JK. Gender differences in social desirability and social approval bias in dietary self-report. Am J Epidemiol. 1997;146:1046–55.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Haycraft EL, Blissett JM. Maternal and paternal controlling feeding practices: reliability and relationships with BMI. Obesity. 2008;16:1552–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Klesges LM, Baranowski T, Beech B, Cullen K, Murray DM, Rochon J, Pratt C. Social desirability bias in self-reported dietary, physical activity and weight concerns measures in 8-to 10-year-old african-american girls: results from the girls health enrichment multisite studies (gems). Prev Med. 2004;38:78–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. McCarney R, Warner J, Iliffe S, Van Haselen R, Griffin M, Fisher P. The hawthorne effect: a randomised, controlled trial. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2007;7:30.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Palfreyman Z, Haycraft E, Meyer C. Parental modelling of eating behaviours: observational validation of the parental modelling of eating behaviours scale (PARM). Appetite. 2015;86:31–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Wall A-M, Hinson RE, McKee SA, Goldstein A. Examining alcohol outcome expectancies in laboratory and naturalistic bar settings: a within-subject experimental analysis. Psychol Addict Behav. 2001;15:219.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Bakeman R, Quera V. Sequential analysis and observational methods for the behavioral sciences. Cambridge University Press; 2011.

  11. Fiese BH, Foley KP, Spagnola M. Routine and ritual elements in family mealtimes: contexts for child well-being and family identity. New Dir Child Adoles. 2006;2006:67–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Mitchell M, Piazza-Waggoner C, Modi A, Janicke D. Examining short-term stability of the mealtime interaction coding system (MICS). J Ped Psychol. 2008;34:63–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. McKenzie TL, Sallis JF, Nader PR, Patterson TL, Elder JP, Berry CC, Rupp JW, Atkins CJ, Buono MJ, Nelson JABEACHES. An observational system for assessing children's eating and physical activity behaviors and associated events. J Appl Behav Anal. 1991;24:141–51.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Johnson SM, Bolstad OD. Methodological issues in naturalistic observation: some problems and solutions for field research. Final report 1972.

  15. Blissett J, Haycraft E. Parental eating disorder symptoms and observations of mealtime interactions with children. J Psychosom Res. 2011;70:368–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Broyles LM, Tate JA, Happ MB. Videorecording in clinical research: mapping the ethical terrain. Nurs Res. 2008;57:59–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Tanofsky-Kraff M, Haynos AF, Kotler LA, Yanovski SZ, Yanovski JA. Laboratory-based studies of eating among children and adolescents. Curr Nutr Food Sci. 2007;3:55–74.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Berkowitz L, Donnerstein E. External validity is more than skin deep: some answers to criticisms of laboratory experiments. Am Psychol. 1982;37:245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Fiese BH, Jones BL, Jarick JM. Family mealtime dynamics and food consumption: an experimental approach to understanding distractions. Couple and Family Psychology: Research and Practice. 2015;4:199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Joyner MA, Kim S, Gearhardt AN. Investigating an incentive-sensitization model of eating behavior: impact of a simulated fast-food laboratory. Clin Psychol Sci 2017:2167702617718828.

  21. Heath C, Hindmarsh J, Luff P. Video in qualitative research. Sage Publications; 2010.

  22. Haidet KK, Tate J, Divirgilio-Thomas D, Kolanowski A, Happ MB. Methods to improve reliability of video-recorded behavioral data. Research in nursing & health. 2009;32:465–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Children's Bureau. Mandatory reporters of child abuse and neglect. Child Welfare Information Gateway 2015. https://www.childwelfare.gov/topics/responding/reporting/mandated/.

  24. Chorney JM, McMurtry CM, Chambers CT, Bakeman R. Developing and modifying behavioral coding schemes in pediatric psychology: a practical guide. J Pediatr Psychol. 2014;40:154–64.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Hulley SB, Cummings SR. Browner WS. Newman TB. Designing clinical research. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins: Grady DG; 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Suen HK, Ary D. Analyzing quantitative behavioral observation data: psychology press; 2014.

  27. Chorney JM, Garcia AM, Berlin KS, Bakeman R, Kain ZN. Time-window sequential analysis: an introduction for pediatric psychologists. J Pediatr Psychol. 2010;35:1061–70.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Noldus LP, Trienes RJ, Hendriksen AH, Jansen H, Jansen RG. The observer video-pro: new software for the collection, management, and presentation of time-structured data from videotapes and digital media files. Behav Res Method. 2000;32:197–206.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Fiese B, Botti J, Greenberg S. ABC family mealtime coding manual. University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign 2007.

  30. Klesges RC, Coates TJ, Brown G, Sturgeon-Tillisch J, Moldenhauer-Klesges LM, Holzer B, Woolfrey J, Vollmer J. Parental influences on children's eating behavior and relative weight. J Appl Behav Anal. 1983;16:371–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Stark LJ, Jelalian E, Powers SW, Mulvihill MM, Opipari LC, Bowen A, Harwood I, Passero MA, Lapey A, Light M. Parent and child mealtime behavior in families of children with cystic fibrosis. J Pedaitr. 2000;136:195–200.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  32. Hughes SO, Patrick H, Power TG, Fisher JO, Anderson CB, Nicklas TA. The impact of child care providers’ feeding on children’s food consumption. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2007;28:100–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Hughes SO, Power TG, Orlet Fisher J, Mueller S, Nicklas TA. Revisiting a neglected construct: parenting styles in a child-feeding context. Appetite. 2005;44:83–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Kiser LJ, Medoff D, Black MM, Nurse W, Fiese BH. Family mealtime q-sort: a measure of mealtime practices. J Fam Psychol. 2010;24:92.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Birch L, Fisher J, Grimm-Thomas K, Markey C, Sawyer R, Johnson S. Confirmatory factor analysis of the child feeding questionnaire: a measure of parental attitudes, beliefs and practices about child feeding and obesity proneness. Appetite. 2001;36:201–10.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Chatoor I, Getson P, Menvielle E, Brasseaux C, O'Donnell R, Rivera Y, Mrazek DA. A feeding scale for research and clinical practice to assess mother—infant interactions in the first three years of life. Inf Mental Hlth J. 1997;18:76–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Melby J, Conger R, Book R, Rueter M, Lucy L, Repinski D, Scaramella L. The Iowa family interaction rating scales. Ames: Iowa State University. Institute for Social and Behavioral Research 1998.

  38. Berge JM, Rowley S, Trofholz A, Hanson C, Rueter M, MacLehose RF, Neumark-Sztainer D. Childhood obesity and interpersonal dynamics during family meals. Pediatrics. 2014;134:923–32.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Dickstein S, Hayden L, Schiller M, Seifer R, San Antonio W. The family mealtime interaction coding system. Unpublished coding manual 1994.

  40. Benson J, Munoz S. Mealtime observation form. Available from author: Unpublished Manual, Syracuse University; 2004.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Berge JM, Jin SW, Hannan P, Neumark-Sztainer D. Structural and interpersonal characteristics of family meals: associations with adolescent body mass index and dietary patterns. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2013;113:816–22.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Jacobs MP, Fiese BH. Family mealtime interactions and overweight children with asthma: potential for compounded risks? J Ped Psychol. 2007;32:64–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Piazza-Waggoner C, Modi AC, Ingerski LM, YP W, Zeller MH. Distress at the dinner table? Observed mealtime interactions among treatment-seeking families of obese children. Childhood. Obesity. 2011;7:385–91.

    Google Scholar 

  44. Sanders M, Le Grice B, Turner K. Mealtime observation schedule: an observers manual. Department of Psychiatry, University of Queensland, Herston, Queensland, Australia: Unpublished technical manual; 1989.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Sanders MR, Patel RK, Le Grice B, Shepherd RW. Children with persistent feeding difficulties: an observational analysis of the feeding interactions of problem and non-problem eaters. Health Psychol. 1993;12:64.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Sanders MR, Dadds MR, Bor W. Contextual analysis of child oppositional and maternal aversive behaviors in families of conduct-disordered and nonproblem children. J Clin Child Pyschol. 1989;18:72–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Hodges EA, Johnson SL, Hughes SO, Hopkinson JM, Butte NF, Fisher JO. Development of the responsiveness to child feeding cues scale. Appetite. 2013;65:210–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Koivisto U-K, Fellenius J, Sjöden P-O. Relations between parental mealtime practices and children's food intake. Appetite. 1994;22:245–58.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Cooke EA, Guss JL, Kissileff HR, Devlin MJ, Walsh BT. Patterns of food selection during binges in women with binge eating disorder. Int J Eat Disord. 1997;22:187–94.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Olvera-Ezzell N, Power TG, Cousins JH. Maternal socialization of children's eating habits: strategies used by obese mexican-american mothers. Child Dev. 1990;61:395–400.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Baumrind D, Black AE. Socialization practices associated with dimensions of competence in preschool boys and girls. Child Dev. 1967:291–327.

  52. Power T, McGrath M, Hughes S, Manire S. Mothers, fathers, and child rearing: socialization during the preschool years. Baltimore, MD: Society for Research in Child Development; 1987.

    Google Scholar 

  53. Fisher J, Birch L, Zhang J, Grusak M, Hughes S. External influences on children’s self-served portions at meals. Int J Obes. 2013;37:954–60.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  54. Iannotti RJ, O'Brien RW, Spillman DM. Parental and peer influences on food consumption of preschool african-american children. Percept Mot Skills. 1994;79:747–52.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Pesch M, Miller A, Appugliese D, Kaciroti N, Rosenblum K, Lumeng J. Low-income mothers' feeding goals predict observed home mealtime and child feeding practices. Child: care, Hlth. Dev. 2016;42:934–40.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  56. Pesch MH, Miller AL, Appugliese DP, Rosenblum KL, Lumeng JC. Affective tone of mothers' statements to restrict their children's eating. Appetite. 2016;103:165–70.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. Power TG, Hughes SO, Goodell LS, Johnson SL, Duran JAJ, Williams K, Beck AD, Frankel LA. Feeding practices of low-income mothers: how do they compare to current recommendations? Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2015;12:34.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  58. Cousins JH, Power TG, Olvera-Ezzell N. Mexican-american mothers′ socialization strategies: effects of education, acculturation, and health locus of control. J Exp Child Psychol. 1993;55:258–76.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

This work was supported by an American Heart Association Fellow-to-Faculty Transition Award to Dr. Pesch (17FTF33630183).

Availability of data and materials

Not applicable.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

MP and JL conceived the manuscript. MP drafted the manuscript and JL edited. Both authors approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Megan H. Pesch.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Pesch, M.H., Lumeng, J.C. Methodological considerations for observational coding of eating and feeding behaviors in children and their families. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 14, 170 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12966-017-0619-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12966-017-0619-3

Keywords