- Research
- Open access
- Published:
Overestimation of physical activity level is associated with lower BMI: a cross-sectional analysis
International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity volume 7, Article number: 68 (2010)
Abstract
Background
Poor recognition of physical inactivity may be an important barrier to healthy behaviour change, but little is known about this phenomenon. We aimed to characterize a high-risk population according to the discrepancies between objective and self-rated physical activity (PA), defined as awareness.
Methods
An exploratory cross-sectional analysis of PA awareness using baseline data collected from 365 ProActive participants between 2001 and 2003 in East Anglia, England. Self-rated PA was defined as 'active' or 'inactive' (assessed via questionnaire). Objective PA was defined according to achievement of guideline activity levels (≥30 minutes or <30 minutes spent at least moderate intensity PA, assessed by heart rate monitoring). Four awareness groups were created: 'Realistic Actives', 'Realistic Inactives', 'Overestimators' and 'Underestimators'. Logistic regression was used to assess associations between awareness group and 17 personal, social and biological correlates.
Results
63.3% of participants (N = 231) were inactive according to objective measurement. Of these, 45.9% rated themselves as active ('Overestimators'). In a multiple logistic regression model adjusted for age and smoking, males (OR = 2.11, 95% CI = 1.12, 3.98), those with lower BMI (OR = 0.89, 95% CI = 0.84, 0.95), younger age at completion of full-time education (OR = 0.83, 95% CI = 0.74, 0.93) and higher general health perception (OR = 1.02 CI = 1.00, 1.04) were more likely to overestimate their PA.
Conclusions
Overestimation of PA is associated with favourable indicators of relative slimness and general health. Feedback about PA levels could help reverse misperceptions.
Background
While the public health importance of physical activity is well established[1, 2], levels of physical activity in the UK have continued to decline and only a third of the population currently meet minimum recommendations[3]. A growing body of research has been directed towards physical activity interventions, but recent reviews show limited evidence of sustained behaviour change and the underlying barriers remain unclear[4–6].
One possible barrier is that sedentary individuals may be unaware of their inactivity. Unlike dichotomous behaviours such as smoking, physical activity spans multiple planned, incidental and habitual activities over a 24-hour period and thresholds of healthy versus unhealthy behaviour may be less clear[7]. This is particularly true of moderate activity (e.g. walking, stair climbing etc), which is often habitual or incidental and may be more difficult to estimate than strenuous activity. Realistic self-assessment depends on accurate recall of the intensity, frequency and duration of physical activity episodes, as well as knowledge of current guidelines and an appropriate definition of physical activity-all requiring high levels of physical activity salience. Evidence from dietary research suggests that summation of this complexity into a single global index may be subject to significant error [8–10], with misperceptions either facilitating (via underestimation) or hindering (via overestimation) behaviour change.
Thresholds of perceived inactivity may also have declined over recent decades, contributing to poor recognition of unhealthy behaviour. Little is known about this issue in relation to physical activity, but international weight perception data suggest that the increased prevalence of obesity over the last decade has been paralleled by a reduction in the ability to self-diagnose overweight [11–13]. Rising inactivity over recent decades may have reduced peoples' ability to distinguish low physical activity levels in a similar way, perhaps creating a faulty social perception that sedentary lifestyles are normal and sufficient. Indeed, work by Lechner et al suggests that Overestimators are more likely to rate their physical activity via comparison with others[14]. With less than 35% of the UK currently active [3], however, such strategies may be misleading.
Evidence to date indicates that more than 60% of adults who do not currently meet recommended guidelines overestimate their level of physical activity, and overestimation is more likely among those with a lower BMI [5]. Moreover, only 27% of overestimators reported a positive intention to change behaviour, compared to 43% among those who accurately assessed their inactivity [5]. Despite being at greatest risk, those who fail to recognise their inactivity are unlikely to perceive a need to change [9, 10] and may therefore be less susceptible to health promotion strategies.
To date, however, misperceptions about physical activity in adults have been assessed by comparing two types of self-report measures; self-rated and quantified self-report [5, 7, 14]. A self-rated measure asks respondents to rate their PA behaviour by selecting one response from a simple scale of options i.e. a single overall summary score of their general PA behaviour. A self-reported measure summarises detailed quantified recollections of PA behaviour over a defined time period (e.g. past week/month/year), usually by means of questionnaires or diaries. Answers to both are used separately to score adherence to PA guidelines [15]. In the past, discrepancies between self-rated and self-reported guideline adherence have been used to determine 'awareness' of physical activity behaviour, and differences with objectively measured physical activity have only been considered in the context of questionnaire error and validity. In this study, the potential discrepancy between objectively measured and self-rated PA is the variable of interest. We are not looking to examine the validity of self-rated versus objective PA, but to examine participants' awareness of the adequacy of their overall PA behaviour (self-rated) compared with objective values. Due to potential error from shared method variance between self-rated and self-reported PA levels, the use of objective physical activity measurement for quantification of PA levels, rather than self-report, would give greater validity to awareness assessment.
Using baseline data from the ProActive cohort [16], we compare objective and self-rated measures of physical activity among sedentary individuals at high-risk of developing diabetes. We undertake an exploratory analysis to examine which personal, social and biological factors are associated with overestimation, and what role psychological variables might play. Results should help confirm whether or not previously reported associations persist when an objective measure of physical activity is used, and will help facilitate identification of Overestimators.
Methods
Study design and participants
In brief, ProActive aimed to evaluate the efficacy of a theory and evidence-based intervention programme to increase physical activity in a self-defined sedentary population. Those aged 30-50 years who had a parental history of diabetes but no known diabetes themselves were eligible to take part. 1521 potentially eligible people were identified via 20 general practices in East Anglia, England [16]. Of these, 1123 completed and returned a brief screening questionnaire based on published measures of occupational and leisure activity [17, 18]; 286 were unwilling to participate and 343 were excluded because they were highly active (as defined by the questionnaire). A further 29 were excluded because either they had been prescribed β blockers that affected heart-rate variability; were unable to walk briskly across flat terrain for 15 min; lived further than 30 min by car from the study centre; or had illness or social obligations that would prevent participation. The remaining 465 potentially eligible participants were screened by telephone to check eligibility and confirm willingness to participate; 31 did not meet inclusion criteria and 35 refused to participate. Although baseline measurements were taken for 399 people, 24 of these participated in the pilot study and a further 10 were excluded prior to randomisation (seven did not meet inclusion criteria, two were unwilling to participate, and one agreed to participate after recruitment had closed). Full details of the original ProActive Trial intervention and protocol have been reported elsewhere [16].
All participants attended one of two measurement centers at baseline, where physiological and anthropometrical measures were taken and participants completed self-administered questionnaires. Immediately post-visit, all participants were measured using individually calibrated heart-rate (HR) monitoring over four consecutive days.
Ethical approval was obtained from the Eastern England MREC and all participants gave written informed consent.
Measures
Awareness
Physical activity awareness was defined as the agreement between self-rated and objectively-measured activity according to current guidelines. Self-rated physical activity was assessed using the following question: 'In general, over the last year would you say you have been: extremely active/moderately active/not very active?' Respondents were classified as 'active' if they answered either 'extremely active' or 'moderately active', and inactive if they answered 'not very active'. Objective free-living physical activity was measured using individually calibrated HR monitoring. The method has been validated against the gold standard techniques of doubly labelled water and whole-body calorimetry [19] and is strongly associated with cardiovascular fitness [20] and the metabolic syndrome [21, 22]. Participants wore HR monitors (Polar Electro, Kemple, Finland) continuously during the waking hours of the four consecutive days post-visit, and were classified as 'active' if they spent more than 30 minutes per day above 1.75 times resting HR (taken as the best approximation of moderate to vigorous physical activity [23]).
In the absence of literature on HR monitoring duration, the choice of a four-day measurement period was based on evidence suggesting that between three and five days of monitoring is necessary to reliably assess habitual objective physical activity levels in adults when using accelerometry [24]. All information was concealed inside the monitor and volunteers did not have access to their HR data at any point. Access and interpretation of the data required specialized software for downloading and processing (available only to the research team). To assess a daily average of minutes of moderate and vigorous physical activity, the fraction of time spent above 1.75 times resting HR was multiplied by the number of minutes recorded and divided by four (the number of wear days). As data on hours worn per day were unavailable, we assumed an average of 12 hours per day for all participants and planned sensitivity analyses to test this assumption. Participants were defined as 'active' if they achieved an average of at least 30 minutes per day (≥ 4.17% of recorded time, calculated as 30 minutes divided by 720 minutes of daily wear time) spent above 1.75 times resting heart rate. Self-rated and objectively-measured physical activity levels were then grouped in a 2 × 2 table to create four awareness categories: 'Realistic Actives', 'Realistic Inactives', 'Overestimators' and 'Underestimators' (Figure 1). The analysis was also repeated with different thresholds (5% either side of the cut-off point) for classifying active versus inactive participants.
Potential correlates of awareness
Psychosocial correlates were measured using a self-administered questionnaire based on the Theory of Planned Behaviour (TPB)[25–27] for which items were selected on the basis of an elicitation study in a similar target group [28]. Perceived behavioural control was assessed using two items: 'it would be difficult for me to be more physically active in the next 12 months even if I wanted to' and 'I am confident that I could be more physically active in the next 12 months, if I wanted to'. Behavioural intention included 'I intend to be more physically active in the next 12 months', and 'it is likely that I will be more physically active in the next 12 months'. Attitude comprised instrumental attitude (measured with two items: 'being more physically active in the next 12 months would be good/harmful for me') and affective attitude (two items: 'for me, being more physically active in the next 12 months would be enjoyable/boring'). Subjective norm was measured using two items: 'most people who are important to me would want me to become more physically active in the next 12 months', and 'most people whose views I value would disapprove if I was more physically active in the next 12 months'. Items were measured on a Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree), based on recommendations by Ajzen [25]. For each variable the scores for negatively formulated items were reversed and a mean score calculated across items for each participant.
Physiological correlates included weight, height and body fat percentage. Weight was measured on standard calibrated scales (SECA; London, UK) and height was determined using a rigid stadiometer. Body mass index (BMI) calculated as weight (kg) divided by height-squared (m2). Body fat percentage was measured by bio-electrical impedance (Bodystat, Isle of Man, UK). To provide an objective measurement of total physical activity energy expenditure, daytime physical activity ratio (DayPar - the ratio of daytime energy expenditure to resting energy expenditure) was also measured using heart rate monitoring with individual calibration for the heart rate-energy expenditure relationship [20].
Age at completion of full-time education, employment status (employed/unemployed), smoking habits (current/former/never) alcohol intake (units per week), and partner's leisure activity (same definitions as for self-rated physical activity) were all measured using a questionnaire developed for the study [16]. General health perception was measured as part of the SF-36 survey using a scale from 0 to 100, whereby higher scores denote more positive perceptions [29].
Statistical analyses
An exploratory analysis was performed using Stata 8.0 software. All data were analysed as continuous variables where possible. To test for overall differences between the four awareness groups, Chi Square tests were applied for categorical variables (gender, employment and smoking groups), and analysis of variance was used for continuous data.
Analyses were undertaken separately for Active participants (Underestimators vs Realistic Actives) and inactive participants (Overestimators vs Realistic Inactives), with a predominant focus on the latter. Associations were modelled using forward-fitting step-wise logistic regression. Successive models were created by adding each independent variable according to ordered blocks; biological (age, sex, BMI, fitness), social/behavioural (education, employment, DayPar, smoking, alcohol intake, and partner's leisure activity over the last year, general health perception). Psychological variables were modelled separately to allow exploration of associations rather than causal pathways. Candidates for each model were identified by significant univariate associations (p ≤ 0.05).
Results
Sample characteristics
The mean age of participants was 40.6 years (SD 6.0) and mean BMI (kg/m2) was 27.8 (SD 5.1). Average daily time spent above 1.75 times resting heart rate showed a skewed distribution, with a mean of 5.64% and a median of 2.45% (SD = 8.28; Range = 0-48.6). Participants were generally overweight (68.2% had a BMI greater than 25), with 28% classified as obese (defined as a BMI of 30 or above), and reported health and anxiety levels comparable to population norms [29, 30]. The majority were female (61.9%) and non-smokers (80.6%). 50.6% were in managerial or professional jobs, and mean age at completion of full-time education was 17.9 years (SD 3).
Of 365 participants, 63.3% were objectively classified as inactive (Figure 1). Almost half of these (45.9%) incorrectly rated themselves as active (Overestimators). In terms of the total sample, 29.0% were classified as Overestimators, 14.3% as Realistic Actives, 34.3% as Realistic Inactives (RI) and 22.5% as Underestimators. Repeating this analysis with a 5% change in threshold either side of the cut-off point did not affect the overall results (data not shown).
Characteristics of awareness groups
Table 1 shows the results of the univariate analysis. Overestimators had a lower BMI and body fat percentage than Realistic Inactives (p < 0.005). Underestimators were the youngest group and had a higher BMI and body fat percentage than either Realistic Actives or Overestimators. A higher percentage of Realistic Actives (30.8%) and Underestimators (29.3%) were smokers, compared to only 9.6% of Realistic Inactives and 17.9% of Overestimators. General health perceptions were highest among Overestimators and lowest among Underestimators. Compared to those who were realistic about their inactivity, Overestimators scored higher on subjective norms.
Table 2 shows the results of multiple logistic regression of awareness on personal, social and biological variables. Among inactive participants, those with a lower BMI, men and smokers were more likely to overestimate their physical activity. Overestimation was also associated with a higher general health perception, a lower age at completion of full-time education, and lower subjective norms. Among active participants (N = 134), underestimation was more likely among those with a higher BMI, lower general health perception and higher subjective norms.
Discussion
In this first study to assess adult awareness using an objective measure of physical activity, we observed that of the 63.3% of the ProActive cohort who were inactive at baseline almost half (45.9%) considered themselves to be active (Overestimators). Furthermore, gender, weight, general health perception and the opinions of significant others about one's physical activity level were all associated with the concordance between self-rated and objectively measured estimates. This may facilitate identification of Overestimators.
Compared to those who correctly rated themselves as inactive, Overestimators had lower BMI on average and tended to have a higher general perception of their health. They were also more likely to be male and less likely to report that significant others in their social environment would like them to become more active. In contrast, participants who underestimated their activity levels were more likely to have a higher BMI and to have a lower general perception of their health compared to those who correctly rated themselves as active, and those underestimating their physical activity level were more likely to report that others would like them to be more active. Given well-known links between weight and physical activity, one possible explanation for these observations is that Overestimators interpret their lower BMI as proof of adequate activity levels, such that overestimation is partly due to 'favourable' anthropometric indicators (whereas the reverse is true for Underestimators) [5, 14]. It would have been preferable to include more predictors of body image and body composition to explore this association further. That this phenomenon persists even in a sedentary population at high risk of type 2 diabetes is particularly noteworthy. However, further work using objectively assessed awareness would be necessary to better to estimate the generalizability of these results. How people feel about their general health may also bias their perception of being active or not. Those who 'feel' healthy might conclude that they do enough activity, while those who rate their overall health more negatively may assume that they are not doing enough. Alternatively, believing one-self to be physically active may by itself prompt feelings of health and well-being, irrespective of objective reality. Longitudinal designs would help explore these associations in more depth.
Men were more likely to overestimate their physical activity than women, possibly reflecting prevailing gender stereotypes i.e. that men are stronger, fitter and thus more 'physical' overall. Overestimators also tended to have spent less time in full-time education, and were less likely to report that significant others in their social environment would like them to become more active. Targeting the 'significant others' of inactive people may therefore offer a novel strategy for promoting physical activity, particularly among men.
Our results replicate previously reported associations between overestimation of physical activity levels, favourable indicators of weight status and lower subjective norms, and suggest that these are unlikely to be due to chance [5, 7, 14].While the strength of previous evidence was limited by its reliance on self-report, the current study demonstrates that these associations persist even when objective behavioural outcomes are used as a benchmark.
Prevalence of overestimation among inactive individuals is slightly lower in the current study (46%) compared to previously published papers, where figures range from 48% to 61% [7, 10]. This may be attributable to the reliance in previous work on self-reported physical activity as the criterion method for defining awareness (since self-reported physical activity tends to produce overestimates of true physical activity), or to the use of different cut-off points for classifying active vs inactive volunteers. Differences in sample characteristics may also have played a role; as part of their recruitment, participants in ProActive were told that they were inactive and at a higher risk of diabetes, but they could reduce their risk by behaviour change. In contrast, two out of three of the previous studies were population-based. It is possible, however, that the discrepancy derives simply from the different criterion measures used (objective vs self-report), and given the greater validity of objective HR monitoring over self-report methods of estimating physical activity, the current estimates may be more reliable.
The strength of this study is the objective measurement of physical activity. Although the 30 minute cut-off point is somewhat artificial, objective physical activity data is likely to reflect true physical activity levels more accurately than self-report measures. It also avoids the problem of correlated error that arises when using two self-report measures. Although we were limited by the absence of data on monitoring duration per day, sensitivity analyses showed that assuming either 10 or 14 hours monitoring per day did not alter the overall findings. It is important to acknowledge, however, that self-rated physical activity was assessed opportunistically using the best measure collected at the time, and that this did not specifically assess participant perception of adherence to the physical activity guidelines assessed with the objective measure. It is unknown which question may best assess physical activity awareness, and which frames of reference are most prevalent in the population. However, as the self-rated physical activity assessment used here has an 'open' frame of reference and leaves interpretation up to the respondent, we feel that this is an appropriate measure given the current evidence base. Although it is likely that perceptions of 'extremely', 'moderately' or 'not very' active are strongly influenced by awareness and knowledge of the guidelines, our findings are however limited by the assumption that the active/inactive distinction for the self-rated and objective measures correspond to each other.
Observed discrepancies between self-rated and objective measures could also reflect a difference in time reference periods (self-rated physical activity over the past year versus objective physical activity over four days), but as these would be equal across groups it is unlikely to have affected overall findings. Discrepancies might also reflect the days of the week or season of the year in which participants were monitored; monitors were attached at the end of the visit and worn over the following 4 days, such that volunteers measured on Mondays and Tuesdays only have weekday free-living data available. Such variations are unlikely to have affected the main findings however, since ProActive participants were recruited and tested throughout the year and clinical visit days were randomly assigned.
We cannot rule out self-selection bias in this study. Participants in a physical activity intervention trial are likely to have had more interest in physical activity than non-responders, and Overestimators and Realistic Actives might have been excluded via the screening questionnaire. While this may have constrained the distribution, the observed range of physical activity was still quite large (mean fraction of time spent above 1.75 times resting HR was 5.6, with values ranging from 0 to 48.6 and a standard deviation of 8.3). Although the number excluded (30%) corresponds well with the proportion designated as active in UK prevalence surveys [3], it is important to keep in mind that ProActive participants were age-restricted and defined as sedentary and at-risk of type 2 diabetes through a parental history of the disease; they were not therefore representative of the general population. As we would expect to see an elevated prevalence of inactivity in ProActive, the true prevalence of overestimation may be higher than our results suggest and more in line with previous findings [5, 7, 14].
Finally, our cross-sectional design precludes the establishment of causality, and prospective cohort studies are recommended for future research. Other unmeasured factors may also play a role. It is also important to recognise that although objective assessment of physical activity has advantages over questionnaire assessment, estimation of this complex behaviour remains a challenge. Reactivity may vary between individuals, for example, and HR monitoring can be susceptible to poor pick-up or interference during free-living conditions [31] and is responsive to triggers such as stress, heat and caffeine. It must also be remembered that a person's heart rate is a physiological response to stimuli, whereas physical activity is a behaviour; although HR monitoring provides a useful guide for estimating physical activity, it is still a proxy measure rather than a direct one.
Public health and research implications
Since public health messages are unlikely to reach those who do not recognise themselves as targets, our findings suggest that up to half of the inactive population may be inaccessible to physical activity interventions. Longitudinal research will help to establish the causal pathways involved, but raising physical activity awareness and adapting public health messages may be an important first step. Future messages could emphasize the benefits of physical activity beyond weight control and stress that inactive people may also be slim, although they are less likely to be healthy. Reversing misperceptions at the population level (i.e. collective versus high-risk interventions) may be the most effective way of reaching Overestimators.
Despite the cross-sectional nature of our findings, it is possible that measurement, awareness and feedback could offer new and interesting avenues for future intervention research. It has been shown that increased self-awareness may improve self-regulation of behaviour[32], and our study aimed to gain more insight into factors which might help us identify ways to help people become more aware of their behaviour. Further work is necessary to assess a) the relationship between awareness and behaviour, and between change in awareness and change in behaviour, and b) the effect of feedback on awareness and behaviour. Measurement and feedback have been associated with more accurate perceptions of behaviour and stronger intentions to change [33, 34], and there are positive indications of behavioural effects. A number of randomised controlled trials reported unexpected physical activity increases in the control group indicative of a measurement effect [35–38], and there is promising evidence that pedometers can facilitate healthy behaviour change [39–41]. As such, measurement and feedback strategies could help Overestimators to recognise their own unhealthy behaviour and recalibrate their perceptions of inactivity. To explore this possibility, future research should aim to measure the effect of measurement and/or feedback on awareness and behaviour change, ideally via a randomised controlled trial.
Conclusions
Almost half of inactive participants in this study incorrectly perceived themselves as 'active'. Overestimation of physical activity, defined here as the discordance between objective and self-rated physical activity, was associated with male sex, lower BMI, younger age at completion of full-time education and higher general health perceptions. These results replicate previously reported associations between physical activity overestimation and favourable indicators of health, and highlight the need for further longitudinal research. Strategies for facilitating realistic self-definition of physical activity level also warrant investigation.
References
Department of Health: At least five a week: Evidence on the impact of physical activity and its relationship to health. A report from the Chief Medical Officer. 2004, --- Either ISSN or Journal title must be supplied..
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services: Physical Activity and health: a report of the Surgeon General. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion. 1996, --- Either ISSN or Journal title must be supplied..
Craig R, Mindell J: Health Survey for England 2006: CVD and risk factors adults, obesity and risk factors children. The NHS Information Centre. 2008
Hillsdon M, Foster C, Cavill N, Crombie H, Naidoo B: The effectiveness of public health interventions for increasing physical activity among adults. A review of reviews. Health Development Agency London. 2005
van Sluijs EMF, Griffin SJ, van Poppel MNM: A cross-sectional study of awareness of physical activity: associations with personal, behavioral and psychosocial factors. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2007, 4: 10.1186/1479-5868-4-53.
van Sluijs EMF, McMinn AM, Griffin SJ: Effectiveness of interventions to promote physical activity in children and adolescents: systematic review of controlled trials. BMJ. 2007, 335: 703-10.1136/bmj.39320.843947.BE.
Ronda G, Van Assema P, Brug J: Stages of change, psychological factors and awareness of physical activity levels in the Netherlands. Health Promot Int. 2001, 16: 305-314. 10.1093/heapro/16.4.305.
Bowen DJ, Fries E, Hopp HP: Effects of dietary fat feedback on behavioral and psychological variables. J Behav Med. 1994, 17: 589-604. 10.1007/BF01857599.
Brug J, van Assema P, Kok GJ, Lenderink T, Glanz K: Self-rated dietary fat intake: association with objective assessment of fat, psychological factors, and intention to change. J Nutr Educ. 1994, 26: 218-223.
Lechner L, Brug J, De Vries H: Misconceptions of fruit and vegetable consumption: differences between objective and subjective estimation of intake. J Nutr Educ. 1997, 29: 313-320. 10.1016/S0022-3182(97)70245-0.
Johnson F, Cooke L, Croker H, Wardle J: Changing perceptions of weight in Great Britain: comparison of two population surveys. BMJ. 2008, 337: a494-10.1136/bmj.a494.
Wardle J, Haase AM, Steptoe A: Body image and weight control in young adults: international comparisons in university students from 22 countries. Int J Obes (Lond). 2006, 30: 644-651. 10.1038/sj.ijo.0803050.
Johnson-Taylor W, Fisher R, Hubbard V, Starke-Reed P, Eggers P: The change in weight perception of weight status among the overweight: comparison of NHANES III (1988-1994) and 1999-2004 NHANES. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2008, 5: 9.
Lechner L, Bolman C, Van Dijke M: Factors related to misperception of physical activity in The Netherlands and implications for health promotion programmes. Health Promot Int. 2006, 21: 104-112. 10.1093/heapro/dal011.
Besson H, Brage S, Jakes RW, Ekelund U, Wareham NJ: Estimating physical activity energy expenditure, sedentary time, and physical activity intensity by self-report in adults. Am J Clin Nutr. 2010, 91: 106-114. 10.3945/ajcn.2009.28432.
Williams K, Prevost AT, Griffin S, Hardeman W, Hollingworth W, Spiegelhalter D, Sutton S, Ekelund U, Wareham N, Kinmonth AL: The ProActive trial protocol - a randomised controlled trial of the efficacy of a family-based, domiciliary intervention programme to increase physical activity among individuals at high risk of diabetes [ISRCTN61323766]. BMC Public Health. 2004, 4: 48-10.1186/1471-2458-4-48.
Godin G, Shephard RJ: A simple method to assess exercise behavior in the community. Can J Appl Sport Sci. 1985, 10: 141-146.
Wareham NJ, Jakes RW, Rennie KL, Schuit J, Mitchell J, Hennings S, Day NE: Validity and repeatability of a simple index derived from the short physical activity questionnaire used in the European Prospective Investigation into Cancer and Nutrition (EPIC) study. Public Health Nutr. 2003, 6: 407-413. 10.1079/PHN2002439.
Rennie KL, Wareham NJ: The validation of physical activity instruments for measuring energy expenditure: problems and pitfalls. Public Health Nutr. 1998, 1: 265-271. 10.1079/PHN19980043.
Wareham NJ, Hennings SJ, Prentice AM, Day NE: Feasibility of heart-rate monitoring to estimate total level and pattern of energy expenditure in a population-based epidemiological study: the Ely Young Cohort Feasibility Study 1994-5. Br J Nutr. 1997, 78: 889-900. 10.1079/BJN19970207.
Simmons RK, Griffin SJ, Steele R, Wareham NJ, Ekelund U: Increasing overall physical activity and aerobic fitness is associated with improvements in metabolic risk: cohort analysis of the ProActive trial. Diabetologia. 2008, 51: 787-794. 10.1007/s00125-008-0949-4.
Wareham NJ, Hennings SJ, Byrne CD, Hales CN, Prentice AM, Day NE: A quantitative analysis of the relationship between habitual energy expenditure, fitness and the metabolic cardiovascular syndrome. Br J Nutr. 1998, 80: 235-241.
Assah FK, Brage S, Ekelund U, Wareham NJ: The association of intensity and overall level of physical activity energy expenditure with a marker of insulin resistance. Diabetologia. 2008, 51: 1399-1407. 10.1007/s00125-008-1033-9.
Trost SG, McIver KL, Pate RR: Conducting accelerometer-based activity assessments in field-based research. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2005, 37: S531-543. 10.1249/01.mss.0000185657.86065.98.
Ajzen I: The Theory of Planned Behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 1991, 50: 179-211. 10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T.
Armitage CJ, Conner M: Efficacy of the Theory of Planned Behaviour: a meta-analytic review. Br J Soc Psychol. 2001, 40: 471-499. 10.1348/014466601164939.
Hardeman W, Kinmonth AL, Michie S, Sutton S, behalf of the ProActive Project Team O: Impact of a physical activity intervention program on cognitive predictors of behaviour among adults at risk of Type 2 diabetes (ProActive randomised controlled trial). International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity. 2009, 6: 16.
Sutton S, French D, Hennings SJ, Mitchell J, Wareham NJ, Griffin S, Hardeman W, Kinmonth AL: Eliciting salient beliefs in research on the theory of planned behaviour: The effect of question wording. Current Psychology. 2003, 22: 234-251. 10.1007/s12144-003-1019-1.
Jenkinson C, Layte R, Wright L, Coulter A: The UKSF-36: an analysis and interpretation manual. 1996, Oxford Health Services Research Unit, University of Oxford
Marteau TM, Bekker H: The development of a six-item short-form of the state scale of the Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI). Br J Clin Psychol. 1992, 31 (Pt 3): 301-306.
Wareham NJ, Rennie KL: The assessment of physical activity in individuals and populations: why try to be more precise about how physical activity is assessed?. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 1998, 22 (Suppl 2): S30-S38.
Baumeister RF, DeWall CN, Ciarocco NJ, Twenge JM: Social exclusion impairs self-regulation. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2005, 88: 589-604. 10.1037/0022-3514.88.4.589.
Brug J, Campbell M, van Assema P: The application and impact of computer-generated personalized nutrition education: a review of the literature. Patient Educ Couns. 1999, 36: 145-156. 10.1016/S0738-3991(98)00131-1.
Proper KI, van der Beek AJ, Hildebrandt VH, Twisk JW, van Mechelen W: Short term effect of feedback on fitness and health measurements on self reported appraisal of the stage of change. Br J Sports Med. 2003, 37: 529-534. 10.1136/bjsm.37.6.529.
Harland J, White M, Drinkwater C, Chinn D, Farr L, Howel D: The Newcastle exercise project: a randomised controlled trial of methods to promote physical activity in primary care. BMJ. 1999, 319: 828-832.
Hillsdon M, Thorogood M, White I, Foster C: Advising people to take more exercise is ineffective: a randomized controlled trial of physical activity promotion in primary care. Int J Epidemiol. 2002, 31: 808-815. 10.1093/ije/31.4.808.
Marcus BH, Bock BC, Pinto BM, Forsyth LH, Roberts MB, Traficante RM: Efficacy of an individualized, motivationally-tailored physical activity intervention. Ann Behav Med. 1998, 20: 174-180. 10.1007/BF02884958.
Burke V, Giangiulio N, Gillam HF, Beilin LJ, Houghton S: Physical activity and nutrition programs for couples: A randomized controlled trial. J Clin Epidemiol. 2003, 56: 421-432. 10.1016/S0895-4356(02)00610-8.
Bravata DM, Smith-Spangler C, Sundaram V, Gienger AL, Lin N, Lewis R, Stave CD, Olkin I, Sirard JR: Using Pedometers to Increase Physical Activity and Improve Health: A Systematic Review. JAMA. 2007, 298: 2296-2304. 10.1001/jama.298.19.2296.
Michie S, Abraham C, Whittington C, McAteer J, Gupta S: Effective techniques in healthy eating and physical activity interventions: a meta-regression. Health Psychol. 2009, 28: 690-701. 10.1037/a0016136.
Tudor-Locke C, Lutes L: Why Do Pedometers Work? A Reflection upon the Factors Related to Successfully Increasing Physical Activity. Sports Medicine. 2009, 39: 981-993. 10.2165/11319600-000000000-00000.
Acknowledgements
The authors are very grateful to all ProActive participants, team members and funders for their contributions to this work. The ProActive study was supported by the UK Medical Research Council (ISRCTN 61323766), UK National Health Service R&D, Royal College of General Practitioners Scientific Foundation and the ProActive Fidelity Study by Diabetes UK (ref. no. RG35259). The ProActive Project Team included Nick Wareham (PI), Simon Griffin (PI), David Spiegelhalter (PI), Kate Williams and Julie Grant (study coordinator and recruitment leads); Ulf Ekelund and Emanuella De Lucia-Rolfe (measurement leads); and Toby Prevost and Tom Fanshawe (statisticians). We also thank Stephen Sharp for statistical advice specific to this analysis.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Additional information
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Authors' contributions
SJG, EMFvS and CW conceived and led the research. All authors contributed to the conception, design and interpretation of data. CW conducted data analyses and wrote the initial manuscript. All authors contributed to the critical revision of draft manuscripts and read and approved the final manuscript.
Authors’ original submitted files for images
Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License ( https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0 ), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
About this article
Cite this article
Watkinson, C., van Sluijs, E.M., Sutton, S. et al. Overestimation of physical activity level is associated with lower BMI: a cross-sectional analysis. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 7, 68 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1186/1479-5868-7-68
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1479-5868-7-68