Skip to main content

Effects of snack portion size on anticipated and experienced hunger, eating enjoyment, and perceived healthiness among children

Abstract

Background

Large portion sizes encourage overconsumption. Prior studies suggest that this may be due to errors in anticipating the effects of portion size, although the studies were limited to adults and energy-dense foods.

Objective

Our aim was to investigate potential anticipation errors related to the effects of portion size on hunger, eating enjoyment, and healthiness ratings among 8-to-11-year-old children, for snacks differing in energy density and healthiness perception, and as a function of initial hunger.

Methods

In a within-subject design, 83 children aged 8 to 11 years old were first asked to anticipate how much they would enjoy, how hungry they would feel after eating, and how healthy it would be to eat a recommended serving size, a 50% larger portion, and a 125% larger portion of brownie or applesauce. Over six subsequent sessions, the children were asked to eat all of each of these portions and then rate their post-intake enjoyment, residual hunger, and healthiness perceptions. We also measured hunger at the beginning of each session.

Results

For both snacks, larger portions reduced anticipated and experienced residual hunger similarly. In contrast, larger portions increased anticipated but not experienced eating enjoyment for both snacks; although larger portions increased anticipated and experienced enjoyment ratings among extremely hungry children. All children under-anticipated how much they would enjoy the smaller portion sizes. Healthiness ratings were unaffected by portion size for both snacks but differed across foods (applesauce vs. brownie).

Conclusions

Children anticipate the effects of portion size on hunger change accurately, overestimate the effects of portion size on eating enjoyment, and rate food healthiness on food type and not portion size. Helping children better anticipate the enjoyment from smaller (recommended) portion sizes and understand that food quantity, not just quality, matters for healthy eating may be a solution to improve portion control.

Background

Like adults, children eat more when they are served larger portions, a phenomenon known as the Portion Size Effect (PSE) [1,2,3,4]. Portion sizes have increased steadily over the past 30 to 40 years, both in the United States and in Europe, particularly for snacks [5, 6]. In England, for example, the size of bagels increased by 24% between 1993 and 2013 [7]. As a result, public health authorities including the World Health Organization [8] and Public Health England [9] have recommended downsizing the food portions given to children. A meta-analysis found that, of all healthy eating nudges, portion downsizing had by far the greatest impact on consumption decisions [10]. Another recent meta-analysis found that increasing the size of food portions given to children aged 2 to 12 years old by 51–100% led to a 13% increase in daily energy intake (standardized mean difference = 0.47 [95% CI: 0.39–0.55], which means that intake increased by 0.47 standard deviations), for both unit and amorphous foods [3] and also underscored the need for more research to understand the PSE mechanism in children.

Achieving the goal of downsizing portions requires understanding the effect – both anticipated and experienced – of snack portion sizes on the drivers of children’s food choices such as hunger changes from pre- to post-intake, eating enjoyment, and healthiness judgments. Indeed, it may be challenging for parents to follow the recommended portion downsizing if children believe that smaller portions will leave them hungry, will be less enjoyable, and will not be healthier than the large portions they have become accustomed to [11]. Studying the effects of portion size on children’s anticipated and experienced hunger changes, eating enjoyment and healthiness also advances understanding of the mechanisms underlying PSE, which are poorly understood in children [12, 13].

Studies among adults have examined the links between portion size and expectations about residual hunger, healthiness, and eating enjoyment [14, 15]. Unsurprisingly, adults expect to be less hungry after eating larger portions [16]. How much portion sizes influence anticipated, and experienced, residual hunger among children remains an open question, however. Studies among adults have shown that the perceived healthiness of a food portion is only weakly influenced by its size, especially when compared to its content [17, 18]. A few studies in children have shown that they can take into account the perceived healthiness of a food in their food choices [19, 20], but the evidence is sparse and the few available studies were not related to food portion size choice. Finally, there is evidence that adults tend to expect larger portions of palatable foods to increase eating enjoyment [21] – and that this anticipation may be erroneous [22, 23]. In a recent study [24], 343 women were randomly allocated to four groups. One group was asked to rate the anticipated eating enjoyment for five increasing portions of brownie, while the other three groups rated their actual eating enjoyment after consuming the smallest, middle, or largest portion, respectively. The authors found evidence of misprediction, in the sense that the participants failed to anticipate that smaller portions would be as enjoyable as larger ones, if not more so. This suggests that PSE may be driven, at least in part, by mispredictions, which are common among adults [25, 26]. Supporting the role of mispredictions when judging portion sizes, a recent study among women found that, although they were aware of eating more when served a larger portion, they underestimated by how much they had increased their consumption [27]. Overall, these findings suggest that people are not fully aware of the effects of portion size on their eating behaviors and their judgments.

Two factors, energy density and hunger, are expected to moderate the relationships between portion size and judgements of residual hunger, eating enjoyment, and healthiness. First, Brunstrom and colleagues [28] showed that the expected satiating properties or estimated calorie content increase linearly with energy density only for low energy-dense foods like fruits and vegetables, whereas the relationships for high energy-dense foods reveal an underestimation. This suggests that errors in anticipating the effects of portion size might vary depending on the energy density of the food being evaluated. Second, there is extensive evidence that hunger at the time of the decision influences portion size perception and choice [29,30,31]. For example, hunger leads people to choose larger portions. It also reduces the extent to which adults choose their portion size based on their expected impact on eating enjoyment and increases their reliance on portion size’s expected impact on their residual hunger after eating the portion [24].

In summary, downsizing portions in children requires first understanding the drivers of children’s portion size perceptions. Yet, we do not know whether children (unlike adults) can accurately anticipate the effects of portion sizes, nor whether the association between anticipations and experiences is similar for foods varying in energy density or whether it varies depending on hunger level. Consequently, we aim to measure the effects of portion size on anticipated (pre-intake) and experienced (post-intake) ratings of residual hunger, eating enjoyment and healthiness among 8-to-11-year-old children for two snacks differing in energy density and perceived healthiness (chocolate brownie and applesauce), taking into account their initial hunger level.

Methods

The present study set up is a within-subject design with 7 measurements conducted over 7 weeks. Details are presented below.

Participants

Children aged 8 to 11 from three primary schools in middle-to-upper-class neighborhoods in Dijon (France) were invited to participate in the study. Exclusion criteria were chronic diseases or food allergies. Parents’ permission to let their child participate in the study was requested by the extra-curricular educators of the schools via a consent form. Children’s oral assent to participate was obtained on the first session. The study was approved by the local ethical committee CPP Est I Bourgogne (2016-A00498–43). We aimed to include 80 participants, a sample size based on the observation that in previous studies conducted among children in the same age range, samples of similar size were large enough to detect differences in hedonic evaluations of foods or in food choices for mid-afternoon snacks [19, 32] in the absence of specific data on portion size evaluation. We enrolled 91 children. In each school, participants were split into groups of three to five, each supervised by one adult experimenter. In recognition of their help, the participating schools were given child-care equipment. 10€ were offered to parents for completing the questionnaires.

Food products

Two foods commonly used for a mid-afternoon snack were chosen [33]. Brossard® chocolate brownie was chosen as the high-calorie, indulgent food (456 kcal/100 g) and Andros® chunky applesauce as the lower-calorie, healthier food (70 kcal/100 g). Their respective nutritional characteristics are detailed in Table 1. These foods were selected based on an existing study, which showed that applesauce is perceived to be healthier than brownie by children from the same age group but is as liked as brownie [34]. A pre-test conducted during an open lab day with 27 children of the same age as those involved in the main study confirmed that both foods were familiar, liked and acceptable afternoon snacks.

Table 1 Study foods: energy (kcal) and quantity (g) for each food and each portion

For each food, three portions were selected. Portion 1 corresponded to the serving size recommended by the manufacturer. Portion 2 was 50% bigger than portion 1. Portion 3 was 50% bigger than portion 2 (e.g., for applesauce, the portions were 100 g, 150 g, and 225 g; for details see Table 1). Hence, the gap between portions 2 and 3 (75 g) was 50% bigger than the gap between portions 1 and 2 (50 g). The geometric increase was chosen to span a large size difference with only three possible sizes and to mimic the typical distribution of portion sizes (e.g., sodas are commonly sold in individual containers of 25 cl, 33 cl, and 50 cl).

Prior research [35] has shown that people are more accurate at estimating increases in portion sizes when this increase occurs in only one dimension (either the height, width, or length of the portion) rather than when all three dimensions increase proportionally. Thus, to ensure that the size increase would be accurately perceived by the children, only one dimension of the portions was changed at a time, the length of the brownie and the height of the applesauce in the transparent container. The pre-tests conducted during an open lab day with children also validated these portion sizes as acceptable and ensured that the children had no difficulty distinguishing the portions.

Study timeline and measurements

Seven mid-afternoon sessions (within-subject design) were conducted in the school canteen during the after-school program, at the time children usually have an afternoon snack (between 4.00 pm and 6.00 pm) over 7 weeks (1 mid-afternoon session each week, see Supplemental Figure 1). At the time of the session, children had not eaten since lunchtime (two lunch services are offered between 11:50 and 13:20, but children eat at the same lunch service each day). The first mid-afternoon session was dedicated to the measure of pre-intake anticipations, and the next six mid-afternoon sessions were dedicated to the post-intake measures. During the first session, the children provided six pre-intake measures (2 foods × 3 portion sizes) for each variable of interest. Half of the children started with the brownie, the other half started with the applesauce. For each food, the portions were presented according to a Latin Square design to balance carryover and order effects. The three portions of each food were presented in a metal box, one at a time, with lids hiding the other two portions (see Supplemental Figure 1). This way, the portions were only visible by the child evaluating them. In order to involve the children in the evaluation tasks, after rating all variables for one food, the children had to indicate which portion among the three they would choose, knowing that they would receive their chosen portion at the end of the session for one of the two foods. At the end of the session, the children were given one of the two foods (determined by a random draw) in their chosen size (half of the children received the brownie, the other half the applesauce).

During the following six sessions (sessions 2 to 7), the children were asked to eat one of the six combinations of the two foods and the three portion sizes entirely before evaluating it. The food (brownie or applesauce) systematically varied from 1 week to the next and the portion size was determined according to a Latin Square design to balance carryover and order effects (portion size was determined at the child level but food type was randomized at the level of groups of 3–5 children who all ate the same food). The portion was placed in the middle of the box used in the first session and the children were instructed to eat it with a spoon without removing it from the box. This way, the portion was only visible to the child eating it. The size of the portion was not explicitly told to the children. The leftovers, if any, were evaluated by weighing the plates/containers before and after consumption using a digital kitchen scale (1 g, Soehnle, Benfeld, Germany) to verify compliance with the instruction to consume the portion entirely.

Each session started with a self-reported measure of initial hunger. Then, the children were asked to rate their anticipated (session 1) or experienced eating enjoyment (sessions 2 to 7) of the food portion. Next, they rated the anticipated (session 1) or experienced (sessions 2 to 7) healthiness of the food portion. Finally, they reported their anticipated residual hunger after eating the entire portion (session 1) or their experienced residual hunger after eating the assigned food portion (sessions 2 to 7). The same visual analog scales (0–13.5 cm) were used for pre-intake anticipated and post-intake experienced measurements, except that the wording was adapted. Hunger and eating enjoyment were measured with the scales adapted from Lange and colleagues [32], and perceived healthiness was measured with a scale adapted from Marty and colleagues [19]. All these scales have been validated for the studied age range. The scales and their exact wording are presented in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Scales to evaluate residual hunger (a), eating enjoyment (b), and healthiness (c), pre-intake and post-intake. The visual analog scales (0–13.5 cm) were translated from French

At the beginning of each of the seven sessions, the same experimenter gave instructions to the children and informed them that, during the session, they could talk to each other about anything except the food they were evaluating. A booklet was provided to each child to write down their answers during each session. The experimenters also explained how to use the three scales, using the instructions provided by the authors of the original scales. After the instructions were given collectively, an experimenter accompanied each group of 3–5 children to explain again the instructions if needed or redirect the discussion if children started to talk about their ratings or about the foods. For all sessions, the experimenter asked the children to remain calm and focused, so that they could win a “silent taster certificate”. This certificate was given to them by the end of the seventh session in exchange for the stars earned by the end of each session, also as a means to thank them for their participation.

At the end of each tasting session (sessions 2–7), children were allowed to draw or engage in other activities to keep them busy until all children had completed their evaluations and/or while the anthropometric measurements were carried out on other children. During sessions 5 and 6, children were weighed to the nearest 0.1 kg using a digital scale (Soehnle, Benfeld, Germany) without shoes. Their height was measured to the nearest 0.1 cm using a stadiometer (Seca, Leicester, Birmingham, UK). All measurements were duplicated and made by an experimenter who had previously been trained by a pediatrician. They were used to calculate body mass index or BMI (weight/height2), then converted into age and sex-specific z-scores (z-BMI) based on the French reference data [36].

Statistical analysis

SPSS version 26.0 was used to conduct statistical analyses. The results are reported as the mean ± SD.

Estimation accuracy: anticipated vs. experienced ratings

The goal of the first analyses was to provide baseline results about the level of accuracy of each child’s prediction (for each rating and each portion size) and to examine whether accuracy differed across foods, and for the specific food portion eaten in session 1 (the role of portion size is examined in subsequent analyses). To do so, a two-step procedure was followed. In a first step, each of the 18 pre-intake ratings provided by each child (anticipated eating enjoyment, residual hunger, and perceived healthiness for a small, medium, and large portion of brownie and applesauce) was categorized as 1) an over-estimation if it was more than 10% above the corresponding post-intake experienced rating, 2) an accurate estimation if it was within 10% of the experienced rating, or 3) as an underestimation if it was at least 10% below the experienced rating. In a second step, we predicted with three logistic regressions (one for residual hunger, one for eating enjoyment and one for perceived healthiness) the likelihood of making an accurate prediction based on food type, on a binary variable indicating whether the portion had been eaten by the child in session 1, and the portion size, used here as a covariate. Similar results were obtained when adding sex, age, and z-BMI as covariates and when using a 5% or 15% accuracy threshold (results not shown).

Portion size effects on children’s anticipated and experienced ratings

The accuracy analyses previously described show how many children were able to be correct when anticipating their post-intake ratings but do not speak to the role of portion size on children’s anticipated and experienced ratings. In order to analyze portion size effects, we estimated three mixed-level regressions, one per dependent variable (residual hunger, eating enjoyment, and perceived healthiness), combining data from both foods and combining pre-intake (i.e., anticipated) and post-intake (i.e., experienced) ratings.

The key independent variable was portion size, which was coded as − 1 for the smallest portion, 0 for the medium portion, and 1.5 for the large portion. This coding captures the actual size differences between the portions and ensures that the main effect of the variables interacting with portion size is estimated for the medium portion. The other independent variables were a binary variable capturing the difference between anticipated and experienced ratings (Rating: coded as 0.5 for anticipated ratings and − 0.5 for experienced ratings), a binary variable capturing the difference between the two foods (Food: coded as 0.5 for applesauce and − 0.5 for brownie), and all their interactions. Based on prior evidence regarding the effect of hunger [24], the models included initial hunger at the time of the session and its interaction with portion size. Moreover, they all controlled for sex, age, and z-BMI, given that these variables might have an effect on caloric requirements, hence on the ratings. Initial hunger, age and z-BMI were mean-centered. Similar results were obtained without the covariates (not shown here). To account for the fact that each child provided multiple observations, we used mixed-model regressions allowing for correlated errors at the participant level.

When these analyses showed that portion size differently influenced anticipated and experienced ratings (which was the case for eating enjoyment), we used spotlight analyses with two alternate centering of portion size (0, 1, 2.5 and − 2.5, − 1.5, 0 for the small, medium, and large portion, respectively) to estimate the difference between anticipated and experienced ratings for the smallest and largest portion size respectively, rather than for the medium portion size, as done in the main analysis [37]. Another spotlight analysis, in which the “Rating” binary variable was set to 0 for experienced and to 1 for anticipated ratings, was used to estimate the effects of portion size on experienced eating enjoyment, rather than its average effect for both anticipated and experienced eating enjoyment.

Finally, when the interaction initial hunger × portion size was significant, separate models were estimated on two sub-groups of children, based on a median split of their average initial hunger. Given the high level of hunger of all children at the time of the sessions (which were conducted between 4.00 pm and 6.00 pm; M = 10.5 ± 1.9 on the 0–13.5 cm visual analog scale for initial hunger), we labeled the group of 42 children with lower hunger level as “hungry” (M = 9.0 ± 1.5, t-test of difference from the 6.5 cm scale mid-point = 11.0, p < 0.001) and the 41 children with higher hunger level as “very hungry” (M = 12.0 ± 0.8, t-test of difference from the 6.5 cm scale mid-point = 46.6, p < 0.001).

Supplemental Table 1 provides food and rating-specific coefficients from 12 separate regressions, one for each food and for each of the two ratings (anticipated and experienced) of the three dependent variables (residual hunger, eating enjoyment and perceived healthiness). Additional analyses were conducted to report choice of food portion made by children at the end of the first session, and to evaluate portion size effects at individual level. Their results are available in the Supplemental analyses 1.

Results

Participants

Out of the 91 children who agreed to participate, data for both foods were excluded for 8 participants (6 children who had missed the first session, one who had refused to provide answers, and one who was absent twice during sessions 2–7), leaving 83 children. In addition, 6 children refused to consume the applesauce. As a result, data were available for 83 children for the brownie and for 77 children for the applesauce. Sample characteristics are detailed in Table 2. Out of the 83 children, 45 were female (54%). Children were 9.5 ± 0.8 years old and their average weight was 32.0 ± 5.5 kg.

Table 2 Characteristics of the participating children (N = 83)

Children consumed all the portions that they were given (defined as leaving less than 1 g of food) in 77.3% of the cases as they were instructed to do. Out of the 83 children, 27 (32.5%) were categorized as completers, meaning that they never left more than 1 g of food across the six eating occasions, and the rest (56 children, 67.5%) were categorized as non-completers. Comparisons of the two groups are reported in Supplemental Analyses 1.1 and showed that non completers were not statistically different from completers for any of the control variables (initial hunger, z-BMI, age and sex); but they differed for one dependent variable, anticipated eating enjoyment, which was higher for completers (C) than for non-completers (NC) (MNC = 9.4 ± 2.0 vs MC = 10.3 ± 1.5, F (1,82) = 4.7, p = 0.03). Leftovers amounted to 4.5% of the weight of the original portion on average, (2.2% for brownie and 6.9% for applesauce, F (1, 479) = 13.3, p < 0.01). Analyses of the portion choices made at the end of the first session are reported in Supplemental Analyses 1.2.

Estimation accuracy: anticipated vs. experienced ratings

We found significant differences across foods in the accuracy of pre-intake ratings (N = 83 for brownie and N = 77 for applesauce, for each variable). Overall, only about one third of the pre-intake ratings were accurate (within 10% of the post-intake ratings), a proportion that varied between 23 and 44% depending on the food and construct (see Fig. 2). Logistic regressions showed statistically significant differences between the two foods in the likelihood of accurate estimations (Wald = 4.24, p = 0.040; Wald = 4.01, p = 0.045; Wald = 7.34, p = 0.007 for residual hunger, eating enjoyment, and perceived healthiness, respectively). Children were more likely to make accurate estimations of residual hunger and eating enjoyment for brownie than for applesauce, but judgements of healthiness were more accurate for applesauce than for brownie.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Accuracy of pre-intake anticipations for residual hunger (top), eating enjoyment (middle), and perceived healthiness (bottom). Percentage of over-estimations (more than 10% above the corresponding experienced rating), accurate estimations (within 10% of the corresponding experienced rating), and under-estimations (at least 10% below the corresponding experienced rating) for each food and rating

Furthermore, the estimations made for the portion of food that children had consumed in session 1 were just as accurate as the other estimations (Wald = 0.05, p = 0.82; Wald = 2.22, p = 0.14; Wald = 0.020, p = 0.88 for residual hunger, eating enjoyment, and perceived healthiness, respectively).

Portion size effects on children’s anticipated and experienced ratings

The accuracy analyses showed that about two-thirds of anticipated ratings were more than 10% off compared than their experienced ratings but are silent about the role of portion size, which is analyzed below.

Residual hunger

As shown in Table 3, larger portion sizes decreased residual hunger (B = − 0.76, t = − 10.72, p < 0.001). Anticipated residual hunger was consistently larger than what was experienced (B = 0.40, t = 2.72, p = 0.007) but the type of rating did not interact with portion size (B = -0.09, t = − 0.65, p = 0.52), indicating that portion size similarly reduced anticipated and experienced residual hunger and had the same effect on both foods.

Table 3 Separate mixed-level regression coefficients (SE) for each of the three dependent variables

Figure 3 plots the mean anticipated and experienced residual hunger for both foods combined. The parallel lines show that the patterns of children’s judgments of how hungry they would feel after imagining eating or actually eating each portion size decreased similarly and linearly with portion size.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Means anticipated and experienced residual hunger, for both brownie and applesauce. The portion sizes represent the recommended serving size, or 1.5 or 2.25 multiple of this size respectively. Anticipated and experienced residual hunger decreased similarly with portion size, for both foods

There were no statistically significant differences between any of the regression coefficients comparing brownie and applesauce (all p’s > 0.11). This showed that portion size influenced anticipated and experienced residual hunger similarly for both foods. Moreover, initial hunger at the time of the session was associated with increased residual hunger (B = 0.54, t = 13.06, p < 0.001), independently of portion size (the interaction effect between initial hunger and portion size was insignificant; B = 0.01, t = 0.26, p = 0.79).

Eating enjoyment

Table 3 shows that larger portion sizes increased eating enjoyment overall (B = 0.34, t = 4.41, p < 0.001), even though anticipated eating enjoyment was consistently below what was experienced (B = -0.68, t = − 4.25, p < 0.001). Importantly, the interaction effect between portion size and anticipated (vs. experienced) rating was positive and statistically significant (B = 0.37, t = 2.38, p = 0.018), indicating that larger portion sizes increased anticipated eating enjoyment more strongly than experienced eating enjoyment. By coding Rating as 0 for experienced rating and 1 for anticipated ratings (rather than − 0.5 and 0.5 respectively), a spotlight analysis revealed that the effect of portion size on experienced eating enjoyment was not statistically significant (B = 0.16, t = 1.43, p = 0.15). As can be seen in Fig. 4a, other spotlight analyses revealed that the difference between anticipated and experienced ratings was statistically different for the smallest portion (B = -1.05, t = − 4.4, p < 0.001) and for the medium portion (B = -0.68, t = − 4.25, p < 0.001 (as shown in Table 3), but not for the largest portion (B = -0.13, t = − 0.5, p = 0.61), indicating that children underestimated how much they would actually enjoy the smaller two portions (but not the largest ones).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Means anticipated and experienced eating enjoyment for both brownie and applesauce among all children (a), very hungry children and hungry children (b). The portion sizes represent the recommended serving size, or 1.5 or 2.25 multiple of this size respectively. Overall, children expected eating enjoyment to increase with portion size, whereas experienced eating enjoyment was independent of portion size. For that reason, the eating enjoyment of the smaller two portions (but not of larger ones) were underestimated. Among very hungry children, portion size increased both anticipated and experienced eating enjoyment

Table 3 further shows that eating enjoyment was lower for applesauce than for brownie (B = -1.29, t = − 7.91, p < 0.001) but none of the interactions involving food type were statistically significant (all p’s > 0.10). There was a significant interaction between portion size and initial hunger (p < 0.001), indicating that initial hunger increased the association between portion size and eating enjoyment. To illustrate this interaction effect, we plotted anticipated and experienced eating enjoyment in two groups of children, dichotomized via a median split on their average initial hunger over the seven sessions (see Fig. 4b). Among very hungry children, portion size increased eating enjoyment (B = 0.64, t = 5.69, p < 0.001), and did so similarly for anticipated and experienced ratings (interaction of portion size and rating: B = 0.36, t = 1.62, p = 0.11). For hungry children, portion size did not influence eating enjoyment (B = 0.03, t = 0.32, p = 0.75), and its effect was not different for anticipated and experienced ratings (interaction of portion size and rating: B = 0.38, t = 1.79, p = 0.07).

Perceived healthiness

The pattern of results was very different for perceived healthiness than for the other two ratings. As Fig. 5 shows, healthiness ratings were significantly higher for applesauce than for brownie (B = 2.50, t = 18.81, p < 0.001) and were barely affected by portion sizes (B = − 0.12, t = − 1.95, p = 0.051). None of the other coefficients was statistically significant (all p’s > 0.11).

Fig. 5
figure 5

Means anticipated and experienced perceived healthiness, for both brownie and applesauce. The portion sizes represent the recommended serving size, or 1.5 or 2.25 multiple of this size respectively. Perceived healthiness is larger for applesauce than for brownie and is unaffected by portion size for both foods

Discussion

The study produced four key results. The first was that the portion sizes of two common food snacks strongly influenced 8-to-11-year-old children’s ratings of hunger and eating enjoyment but not their perception of the healthiness of a portion. Children acknowledged that applesauce was healthier than brownie but judged that eating a 32 g (146 kcal) portion of brownie was not healthier than eating a 125% larger portion of 71 g (328 kcal). This is consistent with prior research showing that elementary school children determine health value based on food groups independently of portion size [38]. These results imply that health-based appeals are unlikely to nudge children to switch to age-appropriate portions, especially considering that smaller portions were judged much less satiating (based on changes in hunger from pre- to post-intake measurements) and less enjoyable to eat than larger portions. This is consistent with the findings of previous studies showing that, in children, enjoyment of food is the first driver of choice [39]. It corroborates the theory that eating pleasure is an effective lever of food choice for children [40].

The second key result is that portion size increased anticipated eating enjoyment but not experienced eating enjoyment. The eating enjoyment ratings provided by the smaller two portions (but not of the largest ones) were underestimated, that is, anticipated enjoyment was below experienced enjoyment for smaller portions. Initial hunger impacted the effects of portion size on eating enjoyment. Among very hungry children, both anticipated and experienced eating enjoyment increased with portion size. Among moderately hungry children, anticipated and experienced eating enjoyment were barely influenced by portion size. In contrast, portion size influenced anticipated and experienced ratings of residual hunger and of healthiness perceptions similarly. This suggests that, when judging portion sizes, children are not subject to mispredictions of hunger changes or of healthiness judgments, but underestimate enjoyment from smaller portions (but not larger ones). Children also fail to anticipate that – provided they are hungry rather than very hungry – eating a 32 g portion of brownie or a 100 g portion of applesauce will be just as enjoyable as eating a portion that is 125% larger. This suggests that helping children better anticipate enjoyment from smaller portion sizes may be a useful track to motivate them to accept and choose smaller snack portions.

The third key result was that, although children rated brownie as more enjoyable to eat than applesauce and rated applesauce as healthier than brownie, portion size influenced anticipated and experienced residual hunger similarly for both foods. In prior research in adults, the expected satiating effect or estimated calorie content of food increased linearly with energy density for foods with low energy density, whereas the relationships for high energy-dense foods reveal an underestimation [28]. Further research is necessary to determine if the discrepancy between our results and previous results regarding the role of calorie density comes from differences between the judgements of adults and children or from the fact that the cognitive processes involved in judging the satiating properties of different foods are not the same as those involved in judging the effects of increasing portion sizes on residual hunger of the same food.

The fourth and final key result was that, unlike previous studies conducted with adults, portion size linearly increased the anticipated eating enjoyment in children for both snacks. In contrast, a study by Cornil and Chandon [24] among adults found that the anticipated eating enjoyment for brownie increased until the middle portion and then decreased slightly, while the experienced eating enjoyment decreased linearly with portion size. This suggests that adults have the ability (unlike children) to accurately forecast that eating enjoyment ultimately decreases with quantity for these kinds of food. It may be that they rely more on cognitive rules when making food judgements than 8- to-11-year-old children, whose cognitive processes with regards to food and eating are still evolving [40, 41]. Of course, it would be of interest to explore this track by comparing children and adults’ judgments in a study specifically designed for that. The observation of a difference between children and adults may be related to the fact that the phenomenon of sensory-specific satiety (i.e. the decline of liking of a specific food over consumption) may manifest itself differently in children and adults [42]. For example, unlike among adults, sensory-specific satiety does not transfer to other foods with similar sensory characteristics in children [42]. Another explanation may be that the children in our study liked large portions of brownie more than the young Parisian women (average age 22 years old) in Cornil and Chandon’s study [24] who may have been more concerned with overeating. On average, the chosen brownie portion was 77% larger in the present study than in their study (298 kcal vs. 168 kcal).

The results of this study should be interpreted in the light of its limitations and strengths. In the absence of prior research on this topic, no power calculation could be run a priori. The sample size was quite small (although it is similar to the sample size used for other studies and reveals significant effects). In addition, contrary to the brownie, applesauce is amorphous so whether the children could distinguish the three portion sizes of applesauce easily as for brownie is a questionable point. However, our finding from the analysis over the group that children were able to predict their residual hunger as accurately for applesauce as for brownie suggests that the steps taken to facilitate the estimation of the portions served were successful.

By asking children to consume the portions served entirely, this study enabled the comparison of pre- and post-intake ratings for the same portion. Still, it would be interesting to explore the robustness of our findings in a different paradigm, where children would be asked to serve themselves, choose their desired portion size and eat ad libitum rather than choose among a selection of fixed portions. This would provide a more granular measure and avoid potential floor or ceiling effects. Future research should also examine apportionable foods [43] or foods sold in small pre-cut portions, such as candies, for which it is the number of units rather than the portion size that drives portion size perception and preferences (for example: [44]). Similarly, investigating the role of the shape of the portions could be interesting. In our study, the portions of brownie only increased in length and the portions of applesauce only in height. Earlier studies show that portions increasing along more than one dimension (e.g., in both width and length) appear to increase more slowly than those that increase in just one dimension [35].

Future studies should also include participants with different characteristics. Our study focused on children who were for the most part 9 or 10 years old; it would be interesting to look at children of different ages, both younger and older. Older children are more likely to also take into account social appeal (linked to the need to support their self-image when socializing) and taste when choosing unhealthy snacks [45]. In addition, given that social norms about portion size and eating habits may vary across different socio-economic groups and that SES is a strong predictor of weight status [46], investigating the same questions in deprived SES groups would be useful. Finally, it is often the parents, not the children themselves, who choose the size of the snacks, and they often disagree about what constitutes a small, medium, or large portion of food [47]. It would therefore be interesting to contrast the anticipations of parents and children regarding the effects of portion size on hunger relief, eating enjoyment, and healthiness judgements.

Conclusion

Overall, this study advances the understanding of how 8-to-11-year-old children evaluate different portion sizes of familiar foods on hunger changes from pre- to post-intake, eating enjoyment, and healthiness, both before and after food intake. Children’s judgments revealed the strong impact of portion size on hunger changes from pre- to post-intake, its small impact on healthiness perception, and its positive impact on anticipated eating enjoyment. However, children did not anticipate the null effect of portion size on eating enjoyment and underestimated the eating enjoyment provided by small and moderate portions.

Studies on children’s portion size-related evaluations are too rare. The present study forms the groundwork for an area of research that will hopefully inform governmental agencies, catering services, food companies and caregivers when making portion sizes decisions for children.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated and/or analyzed during the current study are not publicly available in accordance with the signed consent forms by parents but are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

BMI:

Body Mass Index

References

  1. Kral TVE, Hetherington MM. Variability in children's eating response to portion size. A biobehavioral perspective. Appetite. 2015;88:5–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Hollands GJ, Shemilt I, Marteau TM, Jebb SA, Lewis HB, Wei Y, et al. Portion, package or tableware size for changing selection and consumption of food, alcohol and tobacco. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2015;9:CD011045.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Reale S, Hamilton J, Akparibo R, Hetherington MM, Cecil JE, Caton SJ. The effect of food type on the portion size effect in children aged 2–12 years: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Appetite. 2019;137:47–61.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Vandenbroele J, Van Kerckhove A, Zlatevska N. Portion size effects vary: the size of food units is a bigger problem than the number. Appetite. 2019;140:27–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Nestle M. Increasing portion sizes in american diets: more calories, more obesity. J Am Diet Assoc. 2003;103(1):39–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Matthiessen J, Fagt S, Biltoft-Jensen A, Beck AM, Ovesen L. Size makes a difference. Public Health Nutr. 2003;6(1):65–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Clift J. Portion distortion: how much are we really eating? British Heart Foundation; 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  8. WHO ts. Limiting portion sizes to reduce the risk of childhood overweight and obesity. Biological, behavioural and contextual rationale; 2014. [04/12/2018]; Available from: http://www.who.int/elena/titles/bbc/portion_childhood_obesity/en/.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Tedstone A, Targett V, Mackinlay B, Owtram G, Coulton V, Morgan K, et al. Calorie reduction: the scope and ambition for action. London: Public Health England; 2018.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Cadario R, Chandon P. Which healthy eating nudges work best? A meta-analysis of field experiments. Mark Sci. 2020; https://doi.org/10.1287/mksc.2018.1128.

  11. Hetherington MM, Blundell-Birtill P, Caton SJ, Cecil JE, Evans CE, Rolls BJ, et al. Understanding the science of portion control and the art of downsizing. Proc Nutr Soc. 2018;77(3):347–55.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Steenhuis I, Vermeer W. Portion size: review and framework for interventions. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2009;6(1):58–67.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Marteau TM, Hollands GJ, Shemilt I, Jebb SA. Downsizing: policy options to reduce portion sizes to help tackle obesity. BMJ. 2015;351:h5863.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Labbe D, Rytz A, Godinot N, Ferrage A, Martin N. Is portion size selection associated with expected satiation, perceived healthfulness or expected tastiness? A case study on pizza using a photograph-based computer task. Appetite. 2017;108:311–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Spence M, Stancu V, Dean M, Livingstone MBE, Gibney ER, Lähteenmäki L. Are food-related perceptions associated with meal portion size decisions? A cross-sectional study. Appetite. 2016;103:377–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Brunstrom JM, Collingwood J, Rogers PJ. Perceived volume, expected satiation, and the energy content of self-selected meals. Appetite. 2010;55(1):25–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Liu PJ, Haws KL, Scherr K, Redden JP, Bettman JR, Fitzsimons GJ. The primacy of “what” over “how much”: how type and quantity shape healthiness perceptions of food portions. Manag Sci. 2019;65(7):3353–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Ordabayeva N, Chandon P. In the eye of the beholder: visual biases in package and portion size perceptions. Appetite. 2016;103:450–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Marty L, Nicklaus S, Miguet M, Chambaron S, Monnery-Patris S. When do healthiness and liking drive children's food choices? The influence of social context and weight status. Appetite. 2018;125:466–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Nguyen SP, Girgis H, Robinson J. Predictors of children’s food selection: the role of children's perceptions of the health and taste of foods. Food Qual Prefer. 2015;40:106–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Sorensen LB, Moller P, Flint A, Martens M, Raben A. Effect of sensory perception of foods on appetite and food intake: a review of studies on humans. Int J Obes. 2003;27(10):1152–66.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. Rolls BJ, Rolls ET, Rowe EA, Sweeney K. Sensory specific satiety in man. Physiol Behav. 1981;27(1):137–42.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Rode E, Rozin P, Durlach P. Experienced and remembered pleasure for meals: duration neglect but minimal peak, end (recency) or primacy effects. Appetite. 2007;49(1):18–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Cornil Y, Chandon P. Pleasure as a substitute for size: how multisensory imagery can make people happier with smaller food portions. J Mark Res. 2016;53(5):847–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Galak J, Redden JP. The properties and antecedents of hedonic decline. Annu Rev Psychol. 2018;69(1):1–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Buechel EC, Zhang J, Morewedge CK. Impact bias or underestimation? Outcome specifications predict the direction of affective forecasting errors. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2017;146(5):746–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Keenan GS, Childs L, Rogers PJ, Hetherington MM, Brunstrom JM. The portion size effect: women demonstrate an awareness of eating more than intended when served larger than normal portions. Appetite. 2018;126:54–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Brunstrom J, Drake A, Forde C, Rogers P. Undervalued and ignored: are humans poorly adapted to energy-dense foods? Appetite. 2018;120:589–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Brunstrom J. Mind over platter: pre-meal planning and the control of meal size in humans. Int J Obes. 2014;38:S9–S12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Brunstrom JM, Rogers PJ. How many calories are on our plate? Expected fullness, not liking, determines meal-size selection. Obesity. 2009;17(10):1884–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Brunstrom JM, Shakeshaft NG. Measuring affective (liking) and non-affective (expected satiety) determinants of portion size and food reward. Appetite. 2009;52(1):108–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Lange C, Chabanet C, Nicklaus S, Visalli M, Schwartz C. A dynamic method to measure the evolution of liking during food consumption in 8- to 10-year-old children. Food Qual Prefer. 2019;71:510–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Francou A, Hébel P, Le goûter en perte de vitesse et loin des recommandations, in Consommations et modes de vie 2017, Crédoc. https://www.credoc.fr/download/pdf/4p/290.pdf.

  34. Marty L, Miguet M, Bournez M, Nicklaus S, Chambaron S, Monnery-Patris S. Do hedonic- versus nutrition-based attitudes toward food predict food choices? A cross-sectional study of 6- to 11-year-olds. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2017;14(1):162.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Chandon P, Ordabayeva N. Supersize in one dimension, downsize in three dimensions: effects of spatial dimensionality on size perceptions and preferences. J Mark Res. 2009;46(6):739–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Rolland-Cachera M, Cole T, Sempé M, Tichet J, Rossignol C, Charraud A. Body mass index variations: centiles from birth to 87 years. Eur J Clin Nutr. 1991;45(1):13–21.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Spiller SA, Fitzsimons GJ, Lynch JG Jr, McClelland GH. Spotlights, floodlights, and the magic number zero: simple effects tests in moderated regression. J Mark Res. 2013;50(2):277–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Frerichs L, Intolubbe-Chmil L, Brittin J, Teitelbaum K, Trowbridge M, Huang TTK. Children’s discourse of liked, healthy, and unhealthy foods. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2016;116(8):1323–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Laureati M, Pagliarini E, Toschi TG, Monteleone E. Research challenges and methods to study food preferences in school-aged children: a review of the last 15 years. Food Qual Prefer. 2015;46:92–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Marty L, Chambaron S, Nicklaus S, Monnery-Patris S. Learned pleasure from eating: an opportunity to promote healthy eating in children? Appetite. 2018;120:265–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Schultz CM, Danford CM. Children’s knowledge of eating: an integrative review of the literature. Appetite. 2016;107:534–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Olsen A, Ritz C, Hartvig DL, Møller P. Comparison of sensory specific satiety and sensory specific desires to eat in children and adults. Appetite. 2011;57(1):6–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Almiron-Roig E, Navas-Carretero S, Emery P, Martínez JA. Research into food portion size: methodological aspects and applications. Food Funct. 2018;9(2):715–39.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Ferrage A, Fries LR, Godinot N, Labbe D, Martin N. Fairness-based tasks for assessing children’s perceptions of food quantities and associations with portion selection. Nutrients. 2018;10(4):453.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Tarabashkina L, Quester P, Crouch R. Exploring the moderating effect of children's nutritional knowledge on the relationship between product evaluations and food choice. Soc Sci Med. 2016;149:145–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Claassen MA, Klein O, Bratanova B, Claes N, Corneille O. A systematic review of psychosocial explanations for the relationship between socioeconomic status and body mass index. Appetite. 2019;132:208–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Collins CE, Bucher T, Taylor A, Pezdirc K, Lucas H, Watson J, et al. How big is a food portion? A pilot study in Australian families. Health Promot J Austr. 2015;26(2):83–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank Catherine Pedron, Emilie Szleper, Valérie Feyen, Sylvie Marty, Anaïs Péchadre, Coralie Simard, Margot Arnac for their help with the data collection. We also thank warmly the staff from the city of Dijon and from the three participating schools (Château de Pouilly, Montchapet and Victor Hugo) who allowed us to conduct the study on their premises.

Funding

This work was supported by grants from ANR PUNCH (ANR-15-CE21–0014); the Conseil Régional Bourgogne Franche-Comté (PARI grant) and the FEDER (European Funding for Regional Economic Development).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

The authors’ contributions were as follows: CL, CS, SN, CH, YC, PC designed the research; CH, CL, CS, conducted the research; PC with CL analyzed the data; CS with PC wrote the paper; CL, SN, and YC critically revised the manuscript. All have primary responsibility for the content and approved the final draft.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Camille Schwartz.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study was approved by the local ethical committee CPP Est I Bourgogne (2016-A00498–43).

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

None of the authors have any conflict of interest to declare in relation to this work. The funders had no role in the planning, conduct, or interpretation of the study.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Additional file 1: Figure S1.

Study timeline (from session #1 to session #7 over 7 weeks) and photos of the 3 portion sizes offered for the brownie and the applesauce (presented in metal boxes with compartments and lids).

Additional file 2: Table S1.

Separate mixed model regression coefficients (SE) for each food and each rating of the three dependent variables.

Additional file 3: Analysis S1.

Comparison of children who completely vs. did not completely eat the portions. Choice of food portion made by the children at the end of the first session. Individual-level analyses of the effects of portion sizes on children’s anticipated and experienced ratings.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Schwartz, C., Lange, C., Hachefa, C. et al. Effects of snack portion size on anticipated and experienced hunger, eating enjoyment, and perceived healthiness among children. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 17, 70 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12966-020-00974-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12966-020-00974-z

Keywords