Skip to main content

Impact of physical activity level and dietary fat content on passive overconsumption of energy in non-obese adults

Abstract

Background

Passive overconsumption is the increase in energy intake driven by the high-fat energy-dense food environment. This can be explained in part because dietary fat has a weaker effect on satiation (i.e. process that terminates feeding). Habitually active individuals show improved satiety (i.e. process involved in post-meal suppression of hunger) but any improvement in satiation is unknown. Here we examined whether habitual physical activity mitigates passive overconsumption through enhanced satiation in response to a high-fat meal.

Methods

Twenty-one non-obese individuals with high levels of physical activity (HiPA) and 19 individuals with low levels of physical activity (LoPA) matched for body mass index (mean = 22.8 kg/m2) were recruited. Passive overconsumption was assessed by comparing ad libitum energy intake from covertly manipulated high-fat (HFAT; 50% fat) or high-carbohydrate (HCHO; 70% carbohydrate) meals in a randomized crossover design. Habitual physical activity was assessed using SenseWear accelerometers (SWA). Body composition, resting metabolic rate, eating behaviour traits, fasting appetite-related peptides and hedonic food reward were also measured.

Results

In the whole sample, passive overconsumption was observed with greater energy intake at HFAT compared to HCHO (p < 0.01), without any differences between activity groups (p > 0.05). SWA confirmed that HiPA were more active than LoPA (p < 0.01). HiPA had lower body fat and greater fat-free mass than LoPA (p < 0.05 for both) but did not differ in resting metabolic rate, eating behaviour traits, appetite-related peptides or food reward (p > 0.05 for all).

Conclusions

Non-obese individuals with high or low physical activity levels but matched for BMI showed similar susceptibility to passive overconsumption when consuming an ad libitum high-fat compared to a high-carbohydrate meal. This occurred despite increased total daily energy expenditure and improved body composition in HiPA. Greater differences in body composition and/or physical activity levels may be required to impact on satiation.

Background

There is abundant evidence to support the benefits of habitual physical activity in weight management [1]. Myers et al. have recently shown significant negative associations between objectively-measured moderate-to-vigorous physical activity and markers of adiposity [2]. On the other side of the energy balance equation, the contribution of high-fat energy-dense foods towards obesity cannot be ignored [3, 4]. Passive overconsumption is a global phenomenon and originates from changes in the food supply towards increasingly energy-dense foods, contributing greatly to the obesity epidemic [5]. This is reflected by an unintentional increase in energy intake, arising from a failure to appropriately adjust intake in response to energy density [6]. Control over food intake is strongly influenced by ingestive and post-ingestive feedback from satiation and satiety, two separate aspects of appetite that inhibit eating [7]. Satiation is the process that terminates feeding, measured by the amount of food eaten at a meal, and satiety is the process involved in post-meal suppression of hunger, often measured with a preload-test meal paradigm using preloads differing in energy content [7]. The satiety quotient (SQ), calculated from changes in appetite scores relative to a meal’s energy content [8], can also provide a measure of satiation (immediately after food consumption) and satiety (over a specified amount of time after food consumption) [9]. Dietary fat exerts a weaker effect on satiation within a meal than carbohydrate or protein, and is a key driver of passive overconsumption [6]. For example, in the short-term, when eating ad libitum and to a comfortable level of fullness, individuals consume more calories from high-fat foods compared to high-carbohydrate foods [10–12]. Passive overconsumption is strongly influenced by the higher energy density of fat relative to carbohydrate and protein (9 vs. 4 kcal/g, respectively) [13]. Consequently, eating a high-fat energy-dense diet is conducive to overconsumption and a positive energy balance.

It has been proposed that habitual physical activity improves the sensitivity of the appetite control system [14, 15]. Compared to their inactive counterparts, active individuals decrease their energy intake at an ad libitum test meal following a high-energy preload compared to a low-energy preload [16–19]. However, preload studies preclude us from differentiating between satiation and satiety as separate components of appetite. Additionally, little is known regarding the differences in hedonic mechanisms of appetite control (i.e. food reward and preference for high-fat foods) across different physical activity levels, although research on this topic is emerging [20]. Therefore, this study assessed the satiation response to meals varying in fat and carbohydrate in individuals with high levels of physical activity (HiPA) compared to those with low levels of physical activity (LoPA). It was hypothesized that compared to LoPA, HiPA would: consume less energy in the HFAT condition relative to HCHO condition, show a greater satiation response (SQ), have a reduced hedonic response to high-fat foods in response to HFAT and show lower susceptibility to overconsumption on psychological trait measures.

The study was conducted within a multi-level experimental platform assessing several dimensions of appetite control (e.g. environmental, behavioural, psychological, physiological, and metabolic) [21]. Thus, in addition to measuring the response to passive overconsumption, we sought to examine the effects of physical activity level in several putative determinants of appetite control such as body composition, resting metabolic rate, daily energy expenditure, appetite-related peptides, and eating behaviour traits as secondary outcome measures.

Methods

Participants

Forty non-obese adults (21 HiPA and 19 LoPA) aged 18–55 years were recruited via poster and email lists at the University of Leeds, UK (see Table 1 for participant characteristics). Groups were matched for age, sex and body mass index (BMI). Participants were screened for inclusion based on the following criteria: BMI between 20.0 and 29.9 kg/m2 (to allow for a range of body composition), non-smoker, weight stable (±2 kg for previous 3 months), no change in physical activity over the previous 6 months, not currently dieting, no history of eating disorders, not taking any medication known to affect metabolism or appetite, and acceptance of the study foods. In addition, the short-form of the validated International Physical Activity Questionnaire [22] was used to screen for physical activity levels, with participants only eligible if they engaged in at least 40 min of moderate-to-vigorous physical activity during 4 days or more per week (HiPA), or less than 40 min of moderate-to-vigorous physical activity during 1 day or less per week (LoPA). These criteria were based on a previous study that demonstrated differences in satiety between exercisers and non-exercisers [16], and have been used in subsequent studies [20, 23]. Habitual physical activity was subsequently measured objectively using tri-axial accelerometry (SenseWear Armband (SWA); BodyMedia, Inc; Pittsburgh, USA). The study was approved by the School of Psychology Ethical Committee at the University of Leeds (15–0181). Participants provided written informed consent prior to taking part and were remunerated on completing the study.

Table 1 Group characteristics of HiPA and LoPA participants

Study design

As seen in Fig. 1, following a preliminary assessment, HiPA and LoPA participants underwent 2 laboratory probe days in a quasi-experimental study that included a self-determined fixed breakfast followed by an ad libitum high-fat (HFAT) or high-carbohydrate (HCHO) lunch meal in a randomized crossover design. For the 48 h prior to the three testing sessions, the participants refrained from exercise, and for the 24 h prior, did not consume caffeine or alcohol. On each test day, the participants arrived at the research unit between 07:00 and 09:00 following a 10-h fast (no food or drink except water). Prior to the first meal day, the participants consumed their habitual diet but were required to record their food intake for 24 h in a diary that was provided to them during the preliminary assessment, and replicated their food intake prior to the subsequent meal day. Compliance with these guidelines were verified upon arrival at the laboratory for each testing session.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Experimental protocol. HCHO, high-carbohydrate; HFAT, high-fat; LFPQ, Leeds Food Preference Questionnaire; RMR, resting metabolic rate; VAS, visual analogue scales; VO2max, maximal aerobic capacity

During the 2 meal days, measurements included subjective appetite ratings, hedonic preference (explicit liking and implicit wanting) for high-fat foods, and energy intake at breakfast and at an ad libitum HFAT or HCHO lunch 4 h later. Breakfast was ad libitum on the first meal day and then fixed at the same level of intake on the second meal day. At the end of the first meal day, the participants were fitted with the SWA, which was worn for 7 days. Each meal day was separated by at least 9 days.

Preliminary assessment

Approximately 1 week before the meal days, anthropometrics, body composition (fat mass and fat-free mass), resting metabolic rate, maximal aerobic capacity, eating behaviour traits and fasting appetite-related peptides (leptin, acylated ghrelin, insulin, and glucose) were measured.

Anthropometrics and body composition

Standing height without shoes was measured using a stadiometer (Leicester height measure, SECA; UK). Fat mass, fat-free mass and percentage body fat were estimated via air displacement plethysmography (BodPod, Life Measurement, Inc.; USA) following the manufacturer’s instructions and using the Siri equation [24]. Body mass was obtained from the BodPod. Waist circumference was measured using a measuring tape at the level of the umbilicus.

Resting metabolic rate

Resting metabolic rate (RMR) was measured with an indirect calorimeter fitted with a ventilated hood (GEM, Nutren Technology Ltd; UK) following the guidelines of The American Dietetic Association [25]. Participants were required to remain awake but motionless in a supine position for 40 min. The average of the last 30 min of collection was used to determine RMR. Substrate oxidation (respiratory exchange ratio; RER) was calculated using standard stoichiometric equations [26].

Maximal aerobic capacity

Maximal aerobic capacity (VO2max) was determined using a maximal incremental treadmill test based on the modified Balke protocol [27], with the incline increasing 2% in the first minute and 1% for each additional minute until volitional exhaustion. Expired gases (Vyntus CPX, CareFusion; UK) and heart rate (Polar RS400, Polar; Finland) were measured continuously during the test. Prior to each test, the gas analyser was calibrated using gases of known concentrations while the volume sensor was calibrated automatically by the system at flow values of 2 L/s and 0.2 L/s. The average of the last 20 s of the test was considered VO2max.

Eating behaviour traits

Participants completed four validated psychometric questionnaires to assess eating behaviour traits. The Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire (TFEQ) measures three dimensions of eating behaviour: cognitive control of restraint, disinhibition of eating, and susceptibility to hunger [28]. The Binge Eating Scale (BES) assesses the severity of binge eating [29]. The Yale Food Addiction Scale (YFAS) assesses addictive eating behaviour and significant clinical impairment of distress as a result of overeating [30]. The Control of Eating Questionnaire (CoEQ) is designed to assess the severity and type of food cravings experienced over the previous 7 days [31].

Appetite-related peptides

A fasting blood sample was taken by venepuncture for the assessment of leptin, acylated ghrelin, insulin, and glucose. Blood was drawn in EDTA, serum and fluoride collection tubes. Aprotinin (50 μL/mL blood) was immediately added to the EDTA tube for preservation of ghrelin. Plasma and serum obtained were aliquoted and stored at −70 °C until analysis by the Department of Pathology Research & Development at the Leeds Teaching Hospitals NHS Trust (Leeds, UK). All samples analyses were conducted in one batch. Plasma glucose was analysed with the ADVIA Chemistry Glucose Oxidase Concentrated assay (Siemens Healthcare Diagnostics Inc.; UK), serum insulin with the ADVIA Centaur Insulin assay (Siemens Healthcare Diagnostics Inc.; UK), plasma leptin with the Quantikine Human Leptin Immunoassay ELISA kit (R&D Systems Europe Ltd.; UK) and acylated ghrelin with the Spi Bio Acylated Ghrelin Express Enzyme Immunoassay kit (Bertin Pharma; France). The range for the coefficients of variation for intra-assay precision for glucose, insulin, leptin, and acylated ghrelin are 0.2–0.3%, 3.2–4.6%, 3.0–3.3%, and 5.5–10.3%, respectively. Insulin resistance was calculated via the homeostasis model of risk assessment (HOMA-IR) [32].

Meal days

Test meals

Breakfast during the first meal day was ad libitum with wholegrain cereal, semi-skimmed milk and water served in excess of expected consumption. Coffee or tea was also offered (175 g). Food items were weighed before and after consumption to the nearest 0.1 g and the quantities consumed by each participant were subsequently replicated for the next meal day to make the energy content of the meal individually fixed. Energy intake was calculated using energy equivalents for protein, fat and carbohydrate of 4, 9 and 3.75 kcal/g, respectively, from the manufacturers’ food labels. The participants were allowed to leave the laboratory in between breakfast and lunch but were not allowed to eat or drink any foods except water from the bottle provided.

Lunch was presented in excess of expected consumption and included HFAT or HCHO rice and yoghurt. Water (350 g) was also offered. The ingredients of the meals were covertly manipulated to make them HFAT (1.99 kcal/g, 41.3% carbohydrate, 50.7% fat, 8.0% protein) or HCHO (1.41 kcal/g, 70.8% carbohydrate, 19.5% fat, 9.7% protein) but of similar palatability achieved through pilot testing and confirmed by the participants after each meal (sweetness, savouriness, tastiness, pleasantness; p > 0.05 for all). The HFAT rice dish contained tomato & basil rice, vegetable oil, double cream, and grated cheese, and the yoghurt contained low-fat plain yoghurt, double cream, sugar, and maltodextrin. The HCHO rice dish contained tomato & basil rice, vegetable stock, and semi-skimmed milk, and the yoghurt contained whole-milk plain yoghurt, sugar, and maltodextrin. Participants were instructed to eat as little or as much as they wanted until comfortably full. Food items were weighed before and after consumption, and energy intake was calculated as described above. To examine passive overconsumption while accounting for physical activity level, the difference in energy intake between HFAT and HCHO was calculated as a percentage of total daily energy expenditure (TDEE) obtained from the SWA (see methods below), and labelled passive overconsumption index.

Appetite sensations and hedonic preference for high-fat foods

Subjective appetite sensations were assessed via visual analogue scales for hunger and fullness before and after each meal and at hourly intervals throughout the meal day [33]. The satiety quotient (SQ) [8] was calculated for each condition using energy intake at the respective meals with the following formula:

$$ \mathrm{S}\mathrm{Q}\ \left(\mathrm{mm}/\mathrm{kcal}\right) = \frac{\left(\mathrm{rating}\ \mathrm{before}\ \mathrm{eating}\ \mathrm{episode}\hbox{-} \mathrm{rating}\ \mathrm{after}\ \mathrm{eating}\ \mathrm{episode}\right)}{\mathrm{energy}\ \mathrm{of}\ \mathrm{the}\ \mathrm{food}\ \mathrm{consumed}} \times 100 $$

The Leeds Food Preference Questionnaire was administered before and after lunch to determine scores of implicit wanting and explicit liking for high-fat (>50% energy) and low-fat (<20% energy) foods matched for familiarity, sweetness, protein, and acceptability [34]. Low-fat scores were subtracted from high-fat scores to obtain the fat appeal bias score; thus a positive score indicates greater liking or wanting towards high-fat compared to low-fat foods.

Free-living physical activity and energy expenditure

Free-living physical activity and energy expenditure were measured using the SWA in between the 2 meal days, as previously described [2]. Briefly, the participants were instructed to wear the armband on their non-dominant arm over 7 days for at least 23 h per day (awake and asleep, except for the time around showering, bathing or swimming). Compliance was defined as 5 days of wear (including 1 weekend day) with at least 22 h of verifiable time per day. Proprietary algorithms available in the accompanying software (version 8.0 professional) were used to calculate TDEE, PAL (physical activity level; TDEE/basal metabolic rate), minutes spent sleeping, sedentary (<1.5 METs) or in light (1.5–2.9 METs), moderate (3.0–5.9 METs) and vigorous (≥6.0 METs) physical activity. The SWA has shown good accuracy in estimating free-living TDEE and various intensities of physical activity [35–37].

Statistical analysis

Data are reported as mean ± standard deviation. IBM SPSS for Windows (version 21; USA) was used for statistical analyses. We based our sample size after the study by Long et al. [16] who demonstrated in non-obese individuals a difference in food intake between frequent exercisers and non-exercisers. In this preload-test meal design the difference in food intake between groups was ~400 kcal with an effect size of d = 0.94. We allowed for a similar effect size in the present study and calculated that n = 21 per group would be sufficient to detect a difference in intake under the high-fat condition with 1-β = 0.9 and α = 0.05, one-tailed. A total of 39 participants were included in the final sample (HiPA: 10 males, 10 females; LoPA: 8 males, 11 females), as one male participant in HiPA was excluded due to feeling very unwell during the second meal day. Blood samples for 36 participants (20 HiPA and 16 LoPA) were successfully obtained for glucose, insulin and leptin, and because of technical difficulties with the assay, for 22 participants (12 HiPA and 10 LoPA) for ghrelin. SWA data were compliant in 36 participants (19 HiPA and 17 LoPA; 92% compliance). Independent sample t-tests were used to determine differences in participant characteristics between LoPA and HiPA groups. Differences in passive overconsumption index between LoPA and HiPA groups were examined with an independent sample t-test. Differences in energy intake and SQ were identified with two-way mixed-model ANOVAs, with the between-subject factor of physical activity level (HiPA vs. LoPA) and the within-subject factor of meal condition (HFAT vs. HCHO). Differences in appetite sensations and fat appeal bias were identified with three-way mixed-model ANOVAs, with the between-subject factor of physical activity level (HiPA vs. LoPA) and the within-subject factors of meal condition (HFAT vs. HCHO) and food consumption (pre- vs. post-lunch). Statistical significance was established at p < 0.05 and trends were considered at p ≤ 0.07.

Results

Participant characteristics

Despite there being no group differences in BMI, HiPA had significantly lower body fat and greater fat-free mass and VO2max than LoPA (Table 1). HiPA also had a tendency for lower fat mass and greater RER than LoPA (Table 1). There were no significant differences in eating behaviour traits from the CoEQ, BES, TFEQ or YFAS between HiPA and LoPA (p > 0.05 for all; data not shown), but there was a trend towards greater restraint in HiPA (8.8 ± 5.6) compared to LoPA (6.0 ± 3.6; p = 0.07).

There were no group differences in minutes of SWA wear time or sleep time (Table 2). HiPA and LoPA differed on objectively measured habitual physical activity (Table 2); HiPA had significantly greater number of daily steps, TDEE, light physical activity, moderate-to-vigorous physical activity, PAL, and lower sedentary behaviour than LoPA.

Table 2 Habitual physical activity from the SenseWear Armband

Energy intake and passive overconsumption

There were no significant differences in breakfast energy intake between groups (HiPA: 465 ± 208 kcal vs. LoPA: 395 ± 147 kcal; p = 0.231) or between meal days (p = 0.791). For energy intake at the HFAT and HCHO ad libitum lunch meals (Fig. 2), there was a significant main effect of condition (p < 0.001), such that energy intake was higher in HFAT than HCHO, but no main effect of group or condition by group interaction (p > 0.05 for both). Full sample results can be found in Additional file 1. There were no significant group differences in the passive overconsumption index (HiPA: 12.8 ± 9.9% vs. LoPA: 16.3 ± 10.8%; p = 0.301).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Energy intake at ad libitum high-fat (HFAT) and high-carbohydrate (HCHO) lunch meals. Mean ± standard deviation; black lines represent individual responses; *p < 0.001 (main effect of condition HFAT vs. HCHO). HiPA, high level of physical activity; LoPA, low level of physical activity

Appetite ratings and hedonic preference for high-fat foods

For hunger and fullness throughout the meal day, there was a significant effect of food consumption for both hunger and fullness (p < 0.001), but no effect of condition, group or interactions (p > 0.05 for all; data not shown). For SQ at lunch, there was a significant effect of condition (p < 0.001), with SQ at HCHO being greater than HFAT (Fig. 3), but no effect of group or condition and group interaction (p > 0.05 for both). Full sample results can be found in Additional file 1.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Satiety quotient (SQ) at the ad libitum high-fat (HFAT) and high-carbohydrate (HCHO) lunch meals. Mean ± standard deviation; black lines represent individual responses; *p < 0.001 (main effect of condition HFAT vs. HCHO). HiPA, high level of physical activity; LoPA, low level of physical activity

Liking and wanting fat appeal bias in the hungry and fed state did not differ between conditions, nor were there any condition and group interactions (p > 0.05 for all; Table 3). From pre- to post-lunch, ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of food consumption for liking and wanting (p < 0.001), such that the preference for high-fat foods relative to low-fat foods decreased from the hungry to the fed state, but no main effect of group, condition or interaction effects (p > 0.05 for all; Table 3). Full sample results can be found in Additional file 1.

Table 3 Liking and wanting fat appeal bias scores pre- and post-lunch, and change from pre- to post-lunch

Discussion

This is the first study to investigate satiation and passive overconsumption in individuals with high and low physical activity levels within a multi-level appetite control framework. Our data revealed distinct differences in free-living physical activity and body composition between HiPA and LoPA despite similar BMI. However, for both HiPA and LoPA, the nutritional manipulation of increasing dietary fat (and energy density) led to a similar level of passive overconsumption, with greater energy intake in HFAT compared to HCHO, without any concurrent changes in appetite sensations or preference for high-fat foods in the hungry and fed state.

Physical activity, body fat and appetite control

It is important to emphasise the contribution of low levels of physical activity to the accumulation of body fat. We have shown in a non-obese sample that HiPA have greater fat-free mass and lower fat mass compared to LoPA at the same BMI. This supports recent data from our group that found that sedentary behaviour was positively associated with fat mass, while there was a negative association between moderate-to-vigorous physical activity and fat mass [2]. Over time, there exists a dose–response relationship between physical activity level and body weight, such that low levels of physical activity result in greater gains in body weight (i.e. body fat) [38]. An accumulation of body fat leads to insulin resistance and is proposed to be detrimental to satiety signalling [39, 40]. In inactive overweight and obese individuals, exercise training reduces fat mass [9, 41] and also alters the release of appetite-related peptides [19, 42], improves insulin and leptin sensitivity [43–45], and enhances satiety (measured by the SQ) over several hours after a meal [9]. Thus, regular physical activity could sensitize the appetite control system by driving energy intake (via an increase in resting metabolic rate and energy expenditure) but concomitantly increase postprandial sensations of satiety [46].

This study suggests that, in non-obese individuals, higher levels of habitual physical activity do not mitigate the passive overconsumption response when exposed to a high-fat meal. Interestingly, previous studies conducted in non-obese participants have shown enhanced satiety at higher levels of habitual physical activity without large differences in group characteristics in terms of BMI, eating behaviour traits and insulin sensitivity [16, 17]. Larger disturbances in the putative determinants of appetite control, including body composition, leptin, ghrelin, insulin sensitivity, control over eating, disinhibition, and food reward may be required to affect satiation and result in overconsumption. These differences in findings emphasise the importance of distinguishing between separate appetite-related processes when examining the impact of physical activity on food intake. Based on these observations, we can speculate that habitual physical activity may differentially affect the processes of satiation and satiety. While higher levels of habitual physical activity appear to enhance post-prandial satiety responsiveness, it is possible that factors other than physical activity (e.g. meal characteristics and cognitive factors) have a stronger influence on satiation. That said, it is plausible that a greater accumulation of body fat and/or lower levels of physical activity than seen in the present study may be necessary to dysregulate satiation and impact on meal size.

Physical activity and passive overconsumption

The passive overconsumption paradigm used in this study achieved several outcomes. Firstly, increasing the fat content (and energy density) of a food led to an increase in energy intake. Secondly, non-obese individuals with similar BMI but differing in levels of physical activity have similar satiation response to meals varying in fat. Thirdly, SQ differed across the HFAT and HCHO conditions. This demonstrates that per calorie consumed, fat produced a smaller suppression of hunger at the test meal than carbohydrate. These data corroborate previous studies on passive overconsumption via weak satiation and further illustrate the importance of reducing dietary fat (and energy density) to avoid positive energy balance and ultimately weight gain [6, 13]. Not to undermine the contribution of regular physical activity to energy balance, as it is significant as discussed above, but it exemplifies that diet and activity go hand in hand. Indeed, evidence suggests that higher levels of energy expenditure (i.e. habitual physical activity) are beneficial for the regulation of energy balance [14]. A higher energy flux is also helpful in mitigating episodes of overconsumption and fluctuations in energy intake [47, 48]. For example, Murgatroyd et al. showed that imposing sedentary behaviour and an ad libitum diet containing 60% energy from fat resulted in a daily positive energy balance of approximately 1200 kcal more than a day with imposed exercise [49]. In our sample, free-living TDEE as measured by SWA was significantly greater in HiPA than LoPA (600 kcal more per day). Even when accounting for these differences in TDEE with the PO index, the response to passive overconsumption did not differ (13 vs. 16% of TDEE, respectively). This may have been because energy intake was only measured at one meal.

Previously, Caudwell et al. found that after a 12-week exercise-training intervention (5 days per week, 500 kcal per session), overweight and obese individuals significantly lowered energy intake at a high-energy-density dinner test meal (~4 kcal/g, >50% energy from fat) but not at a low-energy-density dinner test meal (~2.4 kcal/g, <25% energy from fat) [50]. Body fat status may be an important contributor to passive overconsumption as differences in energy intake between lean and obese males have been observed at a test meal following a high-fat high-energy preload compared to a low-fat low-energy preload, where the lean group subsequently compensated for the additional energy from fat whereas the obese group did not [40]. Furthermore, studies comparing appetite control between active and inactive individuals have measured satiety using preload-test meal paradigms, which led to the proposition in a recent systematic review that physically active individuals have an increased sensitivity to the energy density of foods [15]. In light of the results of the current study, in non-obese individuals, it is possible that this effect is attributable to mechanisms mediating satiety but not satiation [15].

In terms of food reward, HiPA and LoPA did not differ in their hedonic preference for high-fat foods (liking and wanting fat appeal bias score) when hungry or after eating the HFAT and HCHO meals. However, a recent study showed differences in other markers of liking and wanting using the Leeds Food Preference Questionnaire between active and inactive males; but the 2 groups were not matched for BMI and differed much more in body composition than the current study [20]. Our data showed that HiPA had a tendency for greater restraint score than LoPA, which suggests more cognitive restriction of food intake. Regardless, both groups behaved similarly at the HFAT and HCHO test meals, highlighting the strong environmental influence of dietary fat on energy intake. Independent effects of fat and energy density in passive overconsumption have been observed. It appears that energy density is a stronger driver of passive overconsumption than fat itself because when the energy density of high-fat and high-carbohydrate meals are matched, energy intake is similar [13, 51]. In fact, Hopkins et al. have recently shown independent and positive associations between energy expenditure (via resting metabolic rate) and energy density with daily energy intake [52].

Limitations

There are a number of limitations to take into account in the present study. Firstly, passive overconsumption was measured using a single meal and limits the extrapolation of findings beyond that meal. Any compensation in the post-ingestive period remains unknown. As previous studies reported differences in satiety between active and inactive individuals [16–19], an effect might have been observed in the hours after consuming the HFAT meal, but this was outside the scope of the present study and needs to be addressed in future studies. Secondly, while objective measurement of physical activity was taken after the participants were included in the study and confirmed distinct physical activity levels between HiPA and LoPA, classification of the groups was based on the IPAQ (self-report) and might have confounded the groups. Other potential confounders not taken into account that may have also affected the results include levels of fat mass, fat-free mass, and dietary restraint. Thirdly, the relatively small number of subjects and large inter-individual variability in responses may have resulted in the study being underpowered to detect significant differences. Furthermore, while it was attempted to match the groups by sex, the final sample included a slightly greater proportion of women in the LoPA group compared to the HiPA group (57% vs. 50%, respectively), which may account for the some of the differences in body composition observed. However, when sex was added as a covariate, the significant differences fat-free mass (p = 0.005) and percentage body fat (p = 0.015) remained, as well as the trend towards a difference in fat mass (p = 0.068).

Conclusions

This study provides evidence to support the beneficial effects of high levels of habitual moderate-to-vigorous physical activity (≥4 days/week) on body composition but did not reveal differences in passive overconsumption between non-obese individuals with high and low levels of physical activity matched for BMI. This may help to clarify the differential role of physical activity level in the distinct processes of satiation and satiety. While satiety appears to be enhanced with higher levels of physical activity [15], it is likely that other factors have a stronger influence on satiation. However, it still remains unknown if the lack of observed effect on satiation in LoPA extends to individuals with a greater accumulation of body fat (obese). Nevertheless, in non-obese individuals, our data suggest that a high-fat meal overpowers any physiologic or behavioural influence of physical activity level on eating behaviour, highlighting the importance of a healthy diet in maintaining adequate appetite control and body weight in an obesogenic food environment.

Abbreviations

BES:

Binge eating scale

BMI:

Body mass index

CoEQ:

Control of Eating Questionnaire

HCHO:

High-carbohydrate

HFAT:

High-fat

HiPA:

High level of physical activity

HOMA-IR:

Homeostasis model of risk assessment – insulin resistance

LoPA:

Low level of physical activity

PA:

Physical activity

PAL:

Physical activity level

RER:

Respiratory exchange ratio

RMR:

Resting metabolic rate

SQ:

Satiety quotient

SWA:

SenseWear Armband

TDEE:

Total daily energy expenditure

TFEQ:

Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire

VO2max :

Maximal aerobic capacity

WC:

Waist circumference

YFAS:

Yale Food Addiction Scale

References

  1. Donnelly JE, Blair SN, Jakicic JM, Manore MM, Rankin JW, Smith BK. American College of Sports Medicine position stand. Appropriate physical activity intervention strategies for weight loss and prevention of weight regain for adults. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2009;41:459–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Myers A, Gibbons C, Finlayson G, Blundell J. Associations among sedentary and active behaviours, body fat and appetite dysregulation: Investigating the myth of physical inactivity and obesity. Br J Sports Med. 2016.

  3. Mendoza JA, Drewnowski A, Christakis DA. Dietary energy density is associated with obesity and the metabolic syndrome in U.S. Adults. Diabetes Care. 2007;30:974–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Vernarelli JA, Mitchell DC, Rolls BJ, Hartman TJ. Dietary energy density is associated with obesity and other biomarkers of chronic disease in US adults. Eur J Nutr. 2015;54:59–65.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Swinburn BA, Sacks G, Hall KD, McPherson K, Finegood DT, Moodie ML, Gortmaker SL. The global obesity pandemic: Shaped by global drivers and local environments. Lancet. 2011;378:804–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Blundell JE, MacDiarmid JI. Fat as a risk factor for overconsumption: Satiation, satiety, and patterns of eating. J Am Diet Assoc. 1997;97:S63–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Blundell JE, De Graaf C, Hulshof T, Jebb S, Livingstone B, Lluch A, Mela D, Salah S, Schuring E, van der Knaap H, Westerterp M. Appetite control: Methodological aspects of the evaluation of foods. Obes Rev. 2010;11:251–70.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Green SM, Delargy HJ, Joanes D, Blundell JE. A satiety quotient: A formulation to assess the satiating effect of food. Appetite. 1997;29:291–304.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. King NA, Caudwell PP, Hopkins M, Stubbs RJ, Naslund E, Blundell JE. Dual-process action of exercise on appetite control: Increase in orexigenic drive but improvement in meal-induced satiety. Am J Clin Nutr. 2009;90:921–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Green SM, Wales JK, Lawton CL, Blundell J. Comparison of high-fat and high-carbohydrate foods in a meal or snack on short-term fat and energy intakes in obese women. Br J Nutr. 2000;84:521–30.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Stubbs RJ, Harden CG, Murgatroyd PR, Prentice A. Covert manipulation of dietary fat and energy density: Effect on substrate flux and food intake in men eating ad libitum. Am J Clin Nutr. 1995;62:316–29.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Stubbs RJ, Ritz P, Coward WA, Prentice A. Covert manipulation of the ratio of dietary fat to carbohydrate and energy density: Effect on food intake and energy balance in free-living men eating ad libitum. Am J Clin Nutr. 1995;62:330–7.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Rolls BJ. The role of energy density in the overconsumption of fat. J Nutr. 2000;130:268S–71S.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Blundell JE. Physical activity and appetite control: Can we close the energy gap? Nutr Bull. 2011;36:356–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Beaulieu K, Hopkins M, Blundell JE, Finlayson G. Does habitual physical activity increase the sensitivity of the appetite control system? A systematic review. Sports Med. 2016;46:1897–919.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Long SJ, Hart K, Morgan LM. The ability of habitual exercise to influence appetite and food intake in response to high- and low-energy preloads in man. Br J Nutr. 2002;87:517–23.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Martins C, Truby H, Morgan LM. Short-term appetite control in response to a 6-week exercise programme in sedentary volunteers. Br J Nutr. 2007;98:834–42.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Van Walleghen EL, Orr JS, Gentile CL, Davy KP, Davy BM. Habitual physical activity differentially affects acute and short-term energy intake regulation in young and older adults. Int J Obes. 2007;31:1277–85.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Martins C, Kulseng B, Rehfeld JF, King NA, Blundell JE. Effect of chronic exercise on appetite control in overweight and obese individuals. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2013;45:805–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Horner KM, Finlayson G, Byrne NM, King NA. Food reward in active compared to inactive men: Roles for gastric emptying and body fat. Physiol Behav. 2016;160:43–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Caudwell P, Gibbons C, Hopkins M, Naslund E, King N, Finlayson G, Blundell J. The influence of physical activity on appetite control: An experimental system to understand the relationship between exercise-induced energy expenditure and energy intake. Proc Nutr Soc. 2011;70:171–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Craig CL, Marshall AL, Sjostrom M, Bauman AE, Booth ML, Ainsworth BE, Pratt M, Ekelund U, Yngve A, Sallis JF, Oja P. International physical activity questionnaire: 12-country reliability and validity. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2003;35:1381–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Horner KM, Byrne NM, Cleghorn GJ, King NA. Influence of habitual physical activity on gastric emptying in healthy males and relationships with body composition and energy expenditure. Br J Nutr. 2015;1–8.

  24. Siri WE. Body composition from fluid spaces and density: Analysis of methods. In: Brozek J, Henschel A, editors. Techniques for measuring body composition. Washington: National Academy of Sciences, National Research Council; 1961. p. 223–44.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Compher C, Frankenfield D, Keim N, Roth-Yousey L. Best practice methods to apply to measurement of resting metabolic rate in adults: A systematic review. J Am Diet Assoc. 2006;106:881–903.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Peronnet F, Massicotte D. Table of nonprotein respiratory quotient: An update. Can J Sport Sci. 1991;16:23–9.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. American College of Sports Medicine. ACSM’s health-related physical fitness assessment manual. 3rd ed. Baltimore: Wolters Kluwer Health/Lippincott Williams & Wilkins Health; 2010.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Stunkard AJ, Messick S. The three-factor eating questionnaire to measure dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger. J Psychosom Res. 1985;29:71–83.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Gormally J, Black S, Daston S, Rardin D. The assessment of binge eating severity among obese persons. Addict Behav. 1982;7:47–55.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Gearhardt AN, Corbin WR, Brownell KD. Preliminary validation of the Yale Food Addiction Scale. Appetite. 2009;52:430–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Hill AJ, Weaver CF, Blundell JE. Food craving, dietary restraint and mood. Appetite. 1991;17:187–97.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Matthews DR, Hosker JP, Rudenski AS, Naylor BA, Treacher DF, Turner RC. Homeostasis model assessment: Insulin resistance and β-cell function from fasting plasma glucose and insulin concentrations in man. Diabetologia. 1985;28:412–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Flint A, Raben A, Blundell JE, Astrup A. Reproducibility, power and validity of visual analogue scales in assessment of appetite sensations in single test meal studies. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 2000;24:38–48.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Finlayson G, King N, Blundell J. The role of implicit wanting in relation to explicit liking and wanting for food: Implications for appetite control. Appetite. 2008;50:120–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Johannsen DL, Calabro MA, Stewart J, Franke W, Rood JC, Welk GJ. Accuracy of armband monitors for measuring daily energy expenditure in healthy adults. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2010;42:2134–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. St-Onge M, Mignault D, Allison DB, Rabasa-Lhoret R. Evaluation of a portable device to measure daily energy expenditure in free-living adults. Am J Clin Nutr. 2007;85:742–9.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Welk GJ, McClain JJ, Eisenmann JC, Wickel EE. Field validation of the MTI Actigraph and BodyMedia armband monitor using the IDEEA monitor. Obesity. 2007;15:918–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Jakicic JM, Marcus BH, Lang W, Janney C. Effect of exercise on 24-month weight loss maintenance in overweight women. Arch Intern Med. 2008;168:1550–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Flint A, Gregersen NT, Gluud LL, Moller BK, Raben A, Tetens I, Verdich C, Astrup A. Associations between postprandial insulin and blood glucose responses, appetite sensations and energy intake in normal weight and overweight individuals: A meta-analysis of test meal studies. Br J Nutr. 2007;98:17–25.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Speechly DP, Buffenstein R. Appetite dysfunction in obese males: Evidence for role of hyperinsulinaemia in passive overconsumption with a high fat diet. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2000;54:225–33.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Caudwell P, Gibbons C, Hopkins M, King N, Finlayson G, Blundell J. No sex difference in body fat in response to supervised and measured exercise. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2013;45:351–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Martins C, Kulseng B, King NA, Holst JJ, Blundell JE. The effects of exercise-induced weight loss on appetite-related peptides and motivation to eat. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2010;95:1609–16.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Goodyear LJ, Kahn BB. Exercise, glucose transport, and insulin sensitivity. Annu Rev Med. 1998;49:235–61.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Dyck DJ. Leptin sensitivity in skeletal muscle is modulated by diet and exercise. Exerc Sport Sci Rev. 2005;33:189–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Steinberg GR, Smith AC, Wormald S, Malenfant P, Collier C, Dyck DJ. Endurance training partially reverses dietary-induced leptin resistance in rodent skeletal muscle. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab. 2004;286:E57–63.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Blundell JE, Gibbons C, Caudwell P, Finlayson G, Hopkins M. Appetite control and energy balance: Impact of exercise. Obes Rev. 2015;16:67–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Hill JO. Understanding and addressing the epidemic of obesity: An energy balance perspective. Endocr Rev. 2006;27:750–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Hume DJ, Yokum S, Stice E. Low energy intake plus low energy expenditure (low energy flux), not energy surfeit, predicts future body fat gain. Am J Clin Nutr. 2016;103:1389–96.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Murgatroyd PR, Goldberg GR, Leahy FE, Gilsenan MB, Prentice AM. Effects of inactivity and diet composition on human energy balance. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 1999;23:1269–75.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Caudwell P, Finlayson G, Gibbons C, Hopkins M, King N, Naslund E, Blundell JE. Resting metabolic rate is associated with hunger, self-determined meal size, and daily energy intake and may represent a marker for appetite. Am J Clin Nutr. 2013;97:7–14.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Stubbs RJ, Harbron CG, Prentice AM. Covert manipulation of the dietary fat to carbohydrate ratio of isoenergetically dense diets: Effect on food intake in feeding men ad libitum. Int J Obes. 1996;20:651–60.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  52. Hopkins M, Finlayson G, Duarte C, Whybrow S, Ritz P, Horgan GW, Blundell JE, Stubbs RJ. Modelling the associations between fat-free mass, resting metabolic rate and energy intake in the context of total energy balance. Int J Obes (Lond). 2016;40:312–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

No external funding was received to conduct this study.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author upon request.

Authors’ contributions

KB, MH, JB and GF designed the research, KB collected the data, KB and GF analysed the data, and all authors contributed to the interpretation of the results and writing of the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study was approved by the University of Leeds School of Psychology Ethical Committee (15–0181). Participants provided written informed consent prior to taking part and were remunerated on completing the study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kristine Beaulieu.

Additional file

Additional file 1:

Full sample results for energy intake, satiety quotient, and liking and wanting fat appeal bias score. (DOCX 16 kb)

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Beaulieu, K., Hopkins, M., Blundell, J. et al. Impact of physical activity level and dietary fat content on passive overconsumption of energy in non-obese adults. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 14, 14 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12966-017-0473-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12966-017-0473-3

Keywords